Original Research Reports

Islam and Politics: A Latent Class Analysis of Indonesian Muslims Based on Political Attitudes and Psychological Determinants

Istiqomah*1,2, Joevarian Hudiyana1, Mirra Noor Milla1, Hamdi Muluk1, Bagus Takwin1

Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 2022, Vol. 10(2), 501–517, https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.7303

Received: 2020-08-02. Accepted: 2021-07-01. Published (VoR): 2022-10-12.

Handling Editors: Ali Mashuri, University of Brawijaya, Malang City, Indonesia; Idhamsyah Eka Putra, Persada Indonesia University, Jakarta, Indonesia; Cristina J. Montiel, Ateneo de Manila University, Quezon City, Philippines

*Corresponding author at: Faculty of Psychology, Universitas Indonesia, 16424, Indonesia. E-mail: istiq.ask@gmail.com

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

This study explored the diversity of Muslim political attitudes by conducting a latent class analysis in the rarely investigated context of Indonesia—the largest Muslim country in the world. We surveyed a total of 1208 Indonesian Muslim participants from eight out of 33 Indonesian provinces. The latent class analysis revealed that there are six clusters of Muslim Individuals based on their political attitudes: Fundamentalist Muslim, Nationalist Muslim, Apolitical Muslim, Hijrah Muslim, Moderate Muslim, and Progressive Muslim. Moreover, we also found several meaningful differences in psychological correlates (right-wing authoritarianism, social dominance orientation, and need for cognitive closure) across the six clusters. Taken together, this study sheds some light upon the diversity of Muslim political attitudes and the psychological tendencies that correspond with such attitudes.

Keywords: Muslim, political attitudes, latent class analysis, religious fundamentalism

As stated in ‘The Clash of Civilizations,' intergroup conflicts in the post-Cold War era are caused by the incompatibility of different identities, such as religion (Huntington, 2000). In the last decade, we've seen 9/11, wars in Afghanistan and Iraq, and the rise of the Islamic State of Iraq and Syria (ISIS). However, some would argue that nations' political systems and dynamics play a greater role than the clash of identities (Fukuyama, 2006; Mearsheimer, 2001). Indeed, Christian people in Western societies may be grouped into diverse categories according to their differing opinions about political issues. Similarly, non-Westerners, such as Muslims, should not be reduced to a single ideology that opposes Western modernism. Historically, Muslims have had many schools of thought, some of which may or may not oppose Western liberal humanist values (Enayat, 2005; Hudiyana et al., 2019). Muslims may also be divided by their support for various local political issues within each country (Albritton, 1999; Huda, 2006). Since previous studies found that Muslim political views are quite diverse globally (Ibrahim, 2006), mapping Muslims in a way that acknowledges their complexity may help us understand their tolerance and intergroup hatred. Ultimately, such mapping may help policymakers address potential social issues such as violent extremism.

This is especially true in the context of Indonesian politics. Indonesia has over 87 percent of Muslims, or 207.176.162 people, making it a Muslim-majority country (Badan Pusat Statistik, 2010). With this in mind, Indonesia is also a non-WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) political context that provides a more representative portrayal of political attitudes in a largest Muslim democracy (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010; Rad, Martingano, & Ginges, 2018). Previous reviews seem to suggest that Indonesian Muslims are divided into political camps (Kersten, 2015; Menchik, 2016; Pepinsky, Liddle, & Mujani, 2018; Shepard, 1987; van Bruinessen, 1999). This leads to the assumption that such diversity may shape the differences in their political attitudes and behavior.

Several political psychological studies have viewed Muslims through the lens of Western politics (e.g. Ayoob, 2009; Ibrahim, 2006). Muslims were either minorities or immigrants in this context. Muslim communities can’t be lumped into a single political group or merely be part of a discrete dichotomy due to the existence of vastly divergent ideologies within Islam (e.g. Liberal or Conservative, Moderate or Extreme). William E. Shepard published a notable study in 1987 proposing a Muslim political attitude typology (Shepard, 1987). He tried to present Islam as a two-dimensional political ideology. His work divides Muslim political ideology into two axes. First, the endorsement of an Islamic totalitarian system guiding all political, economic, and social behavior was defined as "Islamic totalism." The second, Islamic modernity, is defined as the acceptance of new technologies, social systems, and the idea of progress. While such research has provided some insight into the diversity of Muslim political views, more research is needed to determine whether Muslims can be mapped in this way. So, the first agenda is to explore the political diversity of Muslim communities.

The dimensional structure is assumed to be inadequate to map Muslim political diversity. Apart from explaining variations in Muslim diversity based on religious interpretations, it is also necessary to map this diversity based on attitudes towards specific political issues. In this study, the author argues that a more detailed typology of Muslim political ideology in Indonesia is needed based on attitudes toward political issues. For these reasons, this study seeks to empirically investigate whether Muslim communities can be classified into more than one political group based on their beliefs, as well as their interpretations of Islamic doctrine and psychological determinants. We surveyed Muslims in Indonesia, the world's largest Muslim country.

Heterogeneity of Political Attitudes in Indonesian Muslim Communities

Since much of the existing literature focuses on the liberal-conservative spectrum (Duckitt, 2001; Erikson & Tedin, 2016), it is unclear whether this knowledge structure is consistent across all contexts within ordinary people's minds (Feldman & Johnston, 2014). This single ideological spectrum acknowledges only the opposing political attitudes within the highly polarized politics of the United States. Though some researchers added a moderate ideological position in the middle of the spectrum (see Branine & Glover, 1997; Kiley, 2017), it remains as a single political spectrum.

In reality, political ideology varies by context. For example, Feldman and Johnston (2014) found that both liberals and conservatives support a wide range of economic and social policies. Moreover, the terms liberal and conservative are arbitrary in many places (Alford, Funk, & Hibbing, 2008). Many cultures do not recognize such terms because political polarization is not based on political ideology but on political figures (Slater & Arugay, 2018) or policy issues (Kinder & Kalmoe, 2017). In non-US contexts, it may be more appropriate to investigate policy preferences based on specific controversial issues within the local context rather than a single ideological dimension.

Religion is deeply ingrained in Indonesian culture (Geertz, 1976; Muluk, Hudiyana, & Shadiqi, 2018). 95 percent of Indonesians living in their own country said religion is important in their lives (Pew Research Center, 2008). Thus, Indonesians are used to evaluating many aspects of their lives from a religious perspective (Mujani, 2007), including the social and economic functioning of society. Within the Indonesian context, Muslim communities may differ in their support for various controversial policies. The two largest Muslim organizations in Indonesia, Nahdlatul Ulama and Muhammadiyah, have been regarded as moderate Muslims, whose followers hold traditionalist or nationalist political stances that preserve Indonesian local wisdom (Barton, 2014). Other Muslim communities stress the establishment of Pan-Islamism, which seeks to replace Indonesia's democratic system with sharia laws (Menchik, 2014). On the other hand, there are also liberal Muslim communities known for their support of pluralism, the secular system, and liberal humanist values (Ali, 2005; Nurdin, 2005).

These groups' supporters may support different political issues. Shepard (1987) proposes that Muslim political attitudes vary widely (e.g. the endorsement of Islamic influence in politics, their stance on economic policies, as well as the support of modernism in daily life). They may support or oppose issues such as banning alcohol, closing nightclubs and bars, and establishing LGBT rights (Tomsa, 2019; Wieringa, 2015) They may also support or oppose religious involvement in public spheres such as mandating Muslim presidential candidates, basing the state on Islam or using religion to determine public policy and even the death penalty for religious blasphemers. They may also support or oppose socialist versus capitalist fiscal systems (Hadiz, 2018), such as whether the government should provide affordable needs for all citizens, raise the minimum wage, reduce the wealth gap, limit foreign investment in Indonesia, etc.

A few studies have been conducted to map Indonesian political attitudes. Edward Aspinall and his colleagues examined the attitudes of various Indonesian political parties to identify political polarization. There was polarization of political attitudes only on religion vs. secular state, not on economic policy or socially progressive stance (Aspinall, Fossati, Muhtadi, & Warburton, 2018). Similarly, Saiful Mujani and his colleagues empirically demonstrated polarization of attitudes on several issues, such as support for establishing sharia laws in Indonesia (Liddle & Mujani, 2007), support for democracy (Mujani, 2007), economic policies (Pepinsky, Liddle, & Mujani, 2012), and political tolerance (Mujani, 2019; Pepinsky, Liddle, & Mujani 2012). He coined the term ‘democratic Islam' to describe Muslims who oppose changing Indonesia's democratic system and who are more tolerant of other groups (Mujani, 2007). From this perspective, Indonesian Muslims are divided into two groups: Democratic Muslims and Puritan Muslims.

However, due to differing interpretations of Muslim teachings and power positions in their respective political stages, Indonesian Muslims' political attitudes may not be a single spectrum. Some Muslim schools of thought (denominations) embrace modernity and the secular establishment, while others reject it, favoring a more totalitarian Islam (Hudiyana, Putra, Ariyanto, Brama, & Muluk, 2019). This spectrum becomes blurred when we consider that economic endorsements such as capitalism or socialism may not be polarized similarly. To investigate the existence of various Muslim political camps, a cluster analysis that can differentiate political attitudes among Indonesian Muslims is more appropriate. On the basis of Islamic teachings and attitudes towards various political issues, we investigated the latent class structure of the Indonesian Muslim typology.

Psychological Correlates and Political Preferences

Social psychologists have identified dispositional factors that influence political attitudes. Several personalities and dispositional traits have been linked to political ideologies or attitudes. Some scholars argue that personal motivation determines political ideology (Jost, 2006; Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003; Jost, Nosek, & Gosling, 2008). They proposed that individuals with higher epistemic motivation—the desire to reduce uncertainty—were more likely to hold conservative political views aimed at preserving the community's traditional system. Thus, those who are more puritanical about policies may have higher epistemic motivation.

Epistemic motivation is the desire to reduce the threat, ambiguity, and complexity of the social world. The need for cognitive closure (NFCC) is defined as the urge to immediately formulate and maintain opinions in order to avoid confusion and ambiguity, and desiring a definite answer (Higgins & Kruglanski, 1996). A person with a high NFCC has closed-mindedness, avoidance of uncertainty, and intolerance for ambiguity. In spite of being exposed to information that contradicts their prior beliefs, people with a high need for cognitive closure tend to choose definitive answers. Conversely, people with low NFCC tend to draw conclusions by gathering and analyzing new information (Brandt & Reyna, 2010).

Social psychologists have found that right-wing authoritarianism or RWA (Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 1992; Duckitt & Sibley, 2010) and social dominance orientation or SDO (Pratto et al., 1994) predict political ideology or orientation. People with higher RWA scores see the world as more dangerous, while people with higher SDO scores see it as more competitive. RWA promotes traditional and religious values such as adherence to the existing social order and respect for authority and tradition. Meanwhile, individuals with high SDO have political attitudes that justify one group dominating another in society, while group hierarchies are seen as fair.

Previous research has suggested that politics and religion are interchangeable because they stem from the same psychological needs (Friesen, Campbell, & Kay, 2015). (Hennes et al., 2012). Thus, religion as a supreme way of life may be correlated with Muslim political attitudes. Religious teachings are seen as absolute morality and should be manifested in society, leading to political attitudes that conform to literal religious teachings. Previous research has shown that higher intratextual religious fundamentalism could explain the perception of threat (Williamson & Hood, 2014), and thus predicted political attitudes or actions (Beller & Kröger, 2018).

We argue that clusters that favor fundamentalism over secularism tend to be higher in various dimensions SDO, RWA, and NFCC. The rationale behind this assumption is that those with higher RWA and NFCC tend to preserve group purity and avoid threats from other groups (Haidt, 2012; Koleva, Graham, Iyer, Ditto, & Haidt, 2012), as well as desire a more simplistic or orderly understanding of the world (Jost et al., 2007; Muluk & Sumaktoyo, 2010). Those who are higher in SDO tend to support the involvement of Islam in public decision making would establish the dominion of Islam in the society. A possible explanation is that these apolitical individuals tend to justify the system such that they may not wish to pursue a political agenda (Meleady & Vermue, 2019; Reese, Proch, & Cohrs, 2014; Stewart & Tran, 2018).

The Present Study

Based on Islamic teachings and political attitudes, this study attempts to determine whether Indonesian Muslim communities are a single cluster, bi-cluster, or multi-cluster. We didn't predict a certain number of clusters. This could harm the many unexplored possibilities. After identifying the clusters, we analyzed the RWA, SDO, and NFCC scores, as well as demographic data for each cluster.

Method

Participants and Procedure

A total of 1208 participants completed our paper-and-pencil survey (Nfemale = 620, 51.32% with Mage = 34.20, SD = 11.71). At the time of data collection, 62.83% were married while 31.29% were single, and the rest of the participants (5.88%) had been divorced. A total of 62.58% participants were currently employed, while the remaining 37.42% were unemployed. A total of 77.90% participants finished secondary school as their highest educational attainment, while the remaining 22.10% had completed higher education.

A total of 12.50% participants reported monthly incomes below IDR 1 million (about USD 70). 12.33 percent reported monthly household income between IDR 1 million and 2 million, and 26.16 percent reported monthly income between IDR 2 million and 3 million. Meanwhile, 41.31 percent of participants reported monthly incomes between IDR 3 and 4 million. The rest of the participants (7.70%) reported monthly incomes exceeding IDR 5 million. In 2020, the Indonesian Bureau of Statistics (Badan Pusat Statistik, 2020) reported that the poverty threshold was IDR 2 million per month per household. Thus, 24.83 percent of participants were poor. This number may not represent each province equally and thus should not be used as a benchmark for our study's representativeness. Jakarta's minimum wage is IDR 4.4 million, while West Sumatera's is IDR 2.4 million (WageIndicator.org, 2021).

This study's participants are all Muslims. Participants came from eight Indonesian provinces, each with its own Islamic values, views, and histories (Sakai & Fauzia, 2014). Indonesia has 33 provinces. Jakarta, North Sumatera, and East Kalimantan are considered melting pots with high levels of multiculturalism. West Sumatera and East Java are two other provinces that have shaped Indonesian Islamic traditions. The remaining three provinces are Aceh, Banten, and South Sulawesi, all of which have strong Islamic traditions. We surveyed the participants using stratified sampling, which assigned a city from each of the eight provinces at random. Then we went to the cities with our questionnaires. Participants were given questionnaires after signing consent forms. Participants were debriefed after completing the survey.

Measures

Political attitudes were measured by asking participants the extent to which they agreed with specific political issues. Responses were recorded using a 7-point Likert scale (1 = ‘Strongly Disagree’, 7 = ‘Strongly Agree’) for each political issue. A total of twelve political issues were presented: 1. The government should ban alcoholic beverages; 2. The government should close the nightclubs and night bars; 3. The government should not support LGBT rights; 4. This country’s president should be Muslim; 5. The system and constitution of this country should be based on religion; 6. Religion should be the primary consideration when making important public policies; 7. Religious blasphemers should be sentenced to death; 8. The government should provide affordable basic goods to the citizens; 9. The minimum labor wage should be increased; 10. The government should regulate the gap between the rich and the poor; 11. The government should regulate economic competition by limiting access of foreign investments; 12. The government should subsidize free electricity, gas, and fuel to the citizens. All other measures (RWA, SDO, NFCC) were adapted to Bahasa Indonesia using back-to-back translation in order to fulfill the cross-cultural adaptation requirement (Beaton, Bombardier, Guillemin, & Ferraz, 2000).

Islamic interpretation was measured by the Totalism in Islam scale. Totalism in Islam was operationally defined as the support for Islam as a totalitarian way of life in all aspects of society (Shepard, 1987). We specifically created 4-items to measure this variable: (1) “Muslims should base their actions by adhering to the Qur’an and Sharia”; (2) “The current solution to the world’s problems lies in the essential principles of the Qur’an and Hadits”; (3) “The duty of Muslims is to fight for a society with an absolute rule by Islam”; and (4) “The fate of humankind has been determined, so Muslims should believe that the laws of the Qur’an and Sunnah are final and unchangeable”. Participants responded to a Likert Scale of 1-7 (1 = ‘Strongly Disagree’, 7 = ‘Strongly Agree’). The internal consistency score for this measure was α = 0.83.

RWA was measured using the adapted version of the 12-item Right-Wing Authoritarianism scale (Altemeyer, 1996), which was re-validated by Passini (2008). There are three dimensions of RWA: submission, aggression, and conventionalism. Participants responded to a Likert Scale of 1-7 (1 = ‘Strongly Disagree’, 7 = ‘Strongly Agree’). The internal consistency score for the submission dimension was α = 0.91, α = 0.76 for the aggression dimension, and α = 0.81 for the conventionalism dimension.

SDO was measured using the adapted version of the 16-item Social Dominance Orientation scale (Ho et al., 2012). The measure consisted of two dimensions, SDO-D (Dominance) and SDO-E (Egalitarian). Participants responded to a Likert Scale of 1-7 (1 = ‘Strongly Disagree’, 7 = ‘Strongly Agree’). The internal consistency score for SDO-D was α = 0.66, and α = 0.65 for SDO-E.

NFCC was measured using the adapted version of the Need for Closure scale (Roets & Van Hiel, 2011). Participants responded to a Likert Scale of 1-7 (1 = ‘Strongly Disagree’, 7 = ‘Strongly Agree’). The internal consistency score for this measure was α = 0.72.

Data Analysis

There were no missing data for all of the variables. We did not conduct an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) prior to the main analysis because we wanted to map the Muslim diversity according to relevant political attitudes or behavior, not ideology. This allows us to look at behaviors/attitudes without resorting to ideology. This aligns with previous research that looked at specific political attitudes rather than ideology in the Brexit debate (see Lewis & de-Wit, 2019). We also used Latent Class Analysis (LCA) instead of Latent Profile Analysis (LPA) because LCA is considered superior when the indicator variables are assumed to be independent (Williams & Kibowski, 2015). Because we didn't use EFA to find the latent construct, we treated each indicator variable as a standalone. We did attempt to conduct LPA and found seven classes or profiles (see Supplementary Materials D). However, following Occam's razor (simplicity leads to better data description), we chose the model that was easier to interpret and more parsimonious (Domingos, 1999). The seven-class LPA model is less conclusive and interpretable than the six-class LCA model (see the Results section). So we don't use LPA in our subsequent analyses.

We conducted a k-means solution of latent class analysis (Magidson & Vermunt, 2002; Morgan, 2015) with an estimate from the Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) based on the scores of twelve political issues. The BIC estimate was used as a criterion to determine the number of clusters that best fit the data. The BIC estimate informs whether a model (when compared to other models) strikes a better balance between model complexity and explanatory power. A lower value for the resulting cluster or model indicates a better fit (Morgan, 2015). We then analyzed the frequency of response for each cluster in the twelve political attitudes by categorizing the samples in three ordinal categories of attitudes based on their Likert response: 1. Negative (Likert point ‘1’ or ‘2’), 2. Neutral/Moderate (Likert point ‘3’, ‘4’, and ‘5’), and 3. Positive (Likert point ‘6’ and ‘7’).

Finally, we analyzed the difference between the classes based on their mean scores of RWA, SDO, NFCC, Totalism in Islam, as well as age, participants’ income, and participants’ education by conducting analysis of variance (ANOVA). We used ANOVA instead of its multivariate counterpart (MANOVA) for two reasons. First, as suggested by Huberty and Morris (1989), the use of MANOVA to control Type 1 error probability is simply illusory. Second, several literatures oppose the practice of using MANOVA because it has to satisfy several assumptions such as observation independence and homogeneity of covariances (Huang, 2020; Pituch & Stevens, 2015). In the context of MANOVA, violation of observation independence best gained through random sampling is very serious (Pituch & Stevens, 2015). As we did not employ a random sampling method, we chose ANOVA instead.

Results

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for the variables in this study. The best fit latent class solution for political attitudes was a 6-class solution (BIC = 17380.19). The BIC score for 6 class solutions was the lowest. So we concluded that Muslim participants fell into six political clusters. Table 2 and Figure 1 show the complete model fit statistics. In addition, the Lo, Mendell, and Rubin (LMR) test was also conducted and is presented in Supplementary Materials B. From the table, we can infer that the p-value of each class compared to its likelihood was statistically significant in most of the classes. This shows that each model from various classes was acceptable, with the exception of class 18. Contrary to BIC, however, the 7 class solution was more acceptable since it achieved the lowest value. However, consistent with the recommendation from previous work (see Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, 2007), LMR may be less parsimonious as it tends to produce more false-positive results in comparison to BIC. Therefore, we used BIC as our primary estimate.

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics

Variable Range of Score M (SD)
SDO
Dominance (SDO-D) 8 to 56 17.74 (4.21)
Egalitarian (SDO-E) 8 to 56 19.55 (3.33)
RWA
Submission 4 to 28 8.90 (2.77)
Aggression 4 to 28 9.22 (2.87)
Conventionalism 4 to 28 10.66 (2.05)
NFCC 15 to 105 26.22 (4.37)
Totalism in Islam 4 to 28 23.13 (3.36)

Note. SDO = social dominance orientation; RWA = right-wing authoritarianism; NFCC = Need for cognitive closure.

Table 2

Complete Model Fit Statistics for Latent Class Analysis

Class BIC χ2 df Entropy-squared G-squared
2 18248,1979 10540098661634100,00 1159 0,8253 5360,4253
3 17696,3863 11707733808888,10 1134 0,8359 4631,1958
4 17545,7176 492922122,22 1109 0,8611 4303,1090
5 17463,9876 424924190,45 1084 0,8275 4043,9609
6 17380,1923 345526488,28 1059 0,8439 3782,7476
7 17424,8531 645182,516 1034 0,8240 3649,9904
8 17495,4853 576662,80 1009 0,8519 3543,2045
9 17575,7764 145918,86 984 3446,0776
10 17655,3093 341137,02 959 3348,1924
11 17731,0477 105586,81 934 3246,5128
12 17838,5432 54185,74 909 3176,5903
13 17941,6504 166409,64 884 3102,2795
14 18052,6644 75360,62 859 3035,8754
15 18169,2873 89032,24 834 2975,0803

Note. BIC = Bayesian Information Criteria; χ2 = Chi-square; df = degree of freedom.

Click to enlarge
jspp.7303-f1
Figure 1

Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) for Each Number of Clusters

Figure 2 and Supplementary Materials A show the frequency of each attitude category (negative, neutral, positive). Class 1 consisted of people who largely rejected Western influences in their personal lives (e.g. alcohol prohibition, nightclub closures, and LGBT rights), largely endorsed Islamic influence in politics (e.g. a Muslim President, sharia laws in the constitution and public policy, and the death penalty for blasphemers), and mostly support the socialist economic agenda (e.g. rich-poor gap control; the increase in labor minimum wage; subsidy and support of basic needs; limiting the economic competition with foreign investors). We named this class "Fundamentalist Muslim" after Islamist movements' puritanical attitudes. People in Class 2 largely rejected Western influence in their personal lives, endorsed the socialist economic agenda, but were moderate in their support for Islamist political influence in the country. We called this group "Nationalist Muslims" because they favored traditional Indonesian secular political systems over Islamist political systems. Class 3 represented people who were neutral or moderate on most issues (relative to other classes). We named this class "Apolitical Muslim" due to their apparent lack of political conviction.

Click to enlarge
jspp.7303-f2
Figure 2

Frequency Graph for the 12 Political Attitudes Across 6 Classes

Note. For x-axes: 1 = Fundamentalist; 2 = Nationalist; 3 = Apolitical; 4 = Hijrah; 5 = Moderate; and 6 = Progressive.

As for Class 4, it mainly consisted of individuals who rejected the Western influences toward private life and mostly endorsed the establishment of Islamist politics but had a tendency to be moderate in their support towards the socialist (vs. capitalist) economic system. We named this class “Hijrah Muslim”—named after a newly observed Muslim community that showed a tendency to endorse the capitalist economic system but reject Western influence and traditionally secular politics. Class 5 consisted of individuals who showed a more balanced response in most issues, with the exception of issues that are considered normative for Indonesian people (e.g. rejection of LGBT rights; socialist economics). Since these individuals showed a tendency to exhibit more normative attitudes in Indonesian public norms (relative to other classes), we named this class “Moderate Muslim”. Finally, Class 6 included individuals who were less likely to reject Western influence in private life, less likely to endorse Islamist politics (compared to any of the other classes), and mostly endorse the socialist economic system. These attitudes may reflect a more humanist and modern stance, and thus we named this class “Progressive Muslim”.

We then examined whether the scores of Totalism in Islam differed between classes by conducting analysis of variance (ANOVA). Class 1 (Fundamentalist Muslim) was found to possess the highest mean score for Totalism in Islam, while Class 5 (Moderate Muslim) was found to possess the lowest mean score. We found that the six classes indeed possessed different degrees of Totalism in Islam. Tukey HSD post-hoc analyses revealed that Fundamentalist Muslims scored higher in Totalism in Islam compared to Nationalist Muslims, p < .001, 95% CI [-5.45; -2.97], Apolitical Muslims, p < .001, 95% CI [-3.36; -0.76], Hijrah Muslims, p < .001, 95% CI [-3.98; -1.90], Moderate Muslims, p < .001, 95% CI [-7.44; -4.15], and Progressive Muslims, p < .001, 95% CI [-2.76; -0.96]. In other words, Fundamentalist Muslims endorsed the Totalitarian Islamic system in society more than any of the other classes. Meanwhile, Progressive Muslims scored higher compared to Nationalist Muslims, p < .001, 95% CI [-0.99; -3.67], and Moderate Muslims, p < .001, 95% CI [-2.23; -5.64], but not higher in comparison to Apolitical Muslims, p = .998, 95% Confidence Interval [1.18; -1.58], and Hijrah Muslims, p = .072, 95% CI [0.05; -2.21]. Furthermore, Apolitical Muslims scored higher in comparison to Nationalist Muslims, p = .003, 95% CI [-0.49; -3.77], and Moderate Muslims, p < .001, 95% CI [-5.69; -1.79], but not higher in comparison to Hijrah Muslims, p = .529, 95% CI [2.35; -0.59]. Finally, Hijrah Muslims scored higher when compared to Moderate Muslims, p < .001, 95% CI [-4.64; -1.08]. Taken together, these results revealed that Progressive Muslims, Apolitical Muslims and Hijrah Muslims exhibited a tendency to be similar in their endorsement towards Totalitarianism of Islam, while Nationalist Muslims and Moderate Muslims showed a tendency to exhibit the lowest endorsement of Totalitarianism of Islam in Indonesia.

Next, we examined whether individuals across different classes exhibited different degrees of psychological correlates. Table 3 illustrates the mean scores, standard deviations, as well as analysis of variance results for all psychological correlates across the six classes. Post-hoc analyses for all psychological correlates are illustrated in Supplementary Materials C. We found that Class 1 scored higher in NFCC compared to any of the other classes, while there were no observed differences between the remaining classes. This means that those in the Fundamentalist Islam class were more likely to be oriented toward cognitive certainty and order than any of the other classes.

Table 3

Mean Scores, Standard Deviations, and ANOVA Results for Each Class

Variable Class 1 Class 2 Class 3 Class 4 Class 5 Class 6 ANOVA
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F df p
N = 462 N = 138 N = 98 N = 197 N = 54 N = 259
Psychological
RWA Submission 9.71 (2.77) 9.02 (1.7) 7.18 (2.97) 7.1 (2.77) 9.17 (2.03) 9.34 (2.39) 18.36 5, 1202 < .001
RWA Aggression 10.34 (2.59) 8.24 (2.04) 8.33 (3.62) 7.48 (3.12) 8.81 (2.5) 9.47 (2.28) 18.58 5, 1202 < .001
RWA Conventional 11.17 (2) 9.3 (1.97) 10.81 (2.5) 10.82 (1.89) 10.37 (2.1) 10.36 (1.71) 9.13 5, 1202 < .001
SDO-D 18.49 (4.71) 16.51 (3.44) 18.62 (4.08) 16.76 (4.8) 16.38 (2.65) 17.04 (3.11) 10.75 5, 1202 < .001
SDO-E 20.37 (3.59) 18.27 (2.76) 20 (2.91) 18.61 (4.07) 18.42 (2.53) 19.52 (2.81) 14.30 5, 1202 < .001
NFCC 27.91 (4.2) 23.93 (3.44) 24.6 (5.84) 25.61 (3.78) 24.89 (3.94) 25.79 (3.85) 13.63 5, 1202 < .001
Totalism in Islam 24.91 (2.42) 20.72 (2.94) 22.85 (4.59) 21.97 (2.75) 19.11 (4.03) 23.05 (2.73) 38.19 5, 1202 < .001
Demographic
Age 34.2 (11.71) 36.12 (12.15) 35.35 (12.4) 33.97 (10.96) 30.65 (10.54) 32.65 (11.22) 1.37 5, 1202 .232
Education 2.10 (0.65) 1.99 (0.61) 2.18 (0.65) 2 (0.52) 2 (0.58) 2.11 (0.61) 2.17 5, 1202 .055
Income 3.05 (1.20) 2.93 (1.04) 3.07 (1.12) 2.96 (0.92) 2.8 (0.98) 2.89 (1.17) 1.17 5, 1202 .321

Note. N = number of samples in a class; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; ANOVA = analysis of variance; F = F-test for mean difference; df = degree of freedom; p = p-values; RWA = right-wing authoritarianism; SDO-D = social dominance orientation – dominance; SDO-E = social dominance orientation – egalitarian; NFCC = need for cognitive closure; Education was categorized in a 4-point scale (1 = did not finish secondary school, 2 = finished secondary school, 3 = finished diploma or undergraduate degree, and 4 = finished postgraduate and doctoral study); Income was categorized in a 5-point scale (1 = below IDR 1 million, 2 = IDR 1 million – IDR 2 million, 3 = IDR 2 million – IDR 3 million, 4 = IDR 3 million – IDR 5 million, and 5 = above IDR 5 million). Education and income variables were regarded as ordinal measures (instead of nominal).

For the submission dimension of RWA, Fundamentalist Muslims scored higher when compared to Apolitical and Hijrah Muslims, but didn’t score higher in comparison to Nationalist, Moderate, and Progressive Muslims. Nationalist, Moderate, and Progressive Muslims were all found to score higher when compared to Apolitical and Hijrah Muslims. Thus, both Apolitical and Hjrah Muslims were observed to be the lowest in scores when we compared them to the other classes in the submission dimension. As for the aggression dimension of RWA, Fundamentalist Muslims scored higher in comparison to any of the other classes, while Progressive Muslims scored higher only when compared to Nationalist Muslims and Hijrah Muslims. There were no other differences observed for the aggression dimension. Finally, for the conventionalism dimension of RWA, Fundamentalist Muslims scored higher in comparison to Nationalist and Progressive Muslims. Moreover, Nationalist Muslims scored higher in comparison to Apolitical, Hijrah, and Progressive Muslims. These findings suggest that Fundamentalist Muslims were more likely to be oriented toward right-wing aggression. However, Fundamentalist Muslims did not differ in right-wing conventionalism and submission when we compared them to Moderate Muslims. Fundamentalist Muslims scored higher, however, in comparison to Nationalist Muslims and Progressive Muslims in right-wing conventionalism. Fundamentalist Muslims also scored higher when compared to Apolitical Muslims and Hijrah Muslims in right-wing submission.

Meanwhile, for SDO-D or the dominance dimension of SDO, Fundamentalist Muslims scored higher compared to all other classes, with the exception of Apolitical Muslims. Moreover, Apolitical Muslims scored higher when compared to Nationalist, Moderate, Hijrah, and Progressive Muslims. Thus, Fundamentalist and Apolitical Muslims ranked as the highest and the second-highest in the SDO-D dimension respectively. As for SDO-E or the egalitarian dimension, we found a similar pattern as with SDO-D, in which Fundamentalist Muslims scored higher compared to any of the other classes, with the exception of Apolitical Muslims. Though Apolitical Muslims scored higher in SDO-E compared to Nationalist, Moderate, and Hijrah Muslims, they did not score higher compared to Progressive Muslims. This pattern is intriguing because evidently, anti-egalitarian orientation was lower for Nationalist Muslims, Moderate Muslims, and even Hijrah Muslims, in comparison to Progressive Muslims.

Finally, we examined the differences of demographic backgrounds between the six classes. As shown in Table 3, participants’ age, education, and income did not differ across different classes. Gender distribution for Class 1 (χ2 = 0.078, p = .780), Class 3 (χ2 = 0.041, p = .840), Class 4 (χ2 = 1.832, p = .176), Class 5 (χ2 = 0.296, p = .586), and Class 6 (χ2 = 1.116, p = .291) also did not differ. However, the frequency of females in the Nationalist Muslim class was higher in comparison to that of the males (χ2 = 6.522, p = .011). The gender distribution for all classes is illustrated in Figure 3.

Click to enlarge
jspp.7303-f3
Figure 3

The Distribution of Gender for All Classes

Discussion

The latent class analysis of 1208 Indonesian Muslims revealed that Muslims cannot be simply dichotomized. Muslims' political views are more varied. The Bayesian Information Criterion best fit six Indonesian Muslim clusters. We named each cluster: Fundamentalist (Class 1), Nationalist (Class 2), Apolitical (Class 3), Hijrah (Class 4) and Moderate Muslim (Class 5), and Progressive Muslims (Class 6). We looked at the RWA, SDO, and NFCC mean scores for each class and found some interesting patterns. Demographic variables (age, income, education) did not differ for each class except for gender.

Individuals in the Fundamentalist Muslim cluster (Class 1) endorsed the prohibition of alcoholic beverages, the denial of LGBT rights, and the closure of nightlife. These attitudes reflect ultra-conservative tendencies that seek to protect the group's purity from outside threats (Haidt, 2012; Koleva et al., 2012; Skitka & Tetlock, 1993). They also endorsed the establishment of sharia laws in the constitution and public policy and elected a Muslim figure as president, as was the punishment of blasphemers. They consistently outperformed all other classes on the Totalism in Islam scale. The rise of religious fundamentalism, which opposes secularization, may explain their popularity (Stark, 1999; Zubaida, 2005). Fundamentalist Muslims also scored higher on the NFCC, the RWA aggression dimension, and all SDO dimensions. They also tend to have more simplistic cognition (Muluk & Sumaktoyo, 2010), a stronger need for certainty and order (Jost et al., 2007), hostility towards weaker minority groups (Duckitt, 2006), and a higher tendency to endorse intergroup hostility (Duckitt, 2006). (De Zavala, Cislak, & Wesolowska, 2010).

They (Class 1) also strongly oppose the capitalist economic system. This finding appears to contradict prior research showing fundamentalist groups were motivated by symbolic rather than economic threats (Jackson & Esses, 1997; Mashuri et al., 2016). However, we argue that in Indonesian politics, Chinese Christian minorities wield economic power within a capitalist financial system, opposing Muslim majorities who resent the aforementioned minority group. Like Amy Chua's "World on Fire" hypothesis, Southeast Asia's free market democracy may breed intergroup hatred (Chua, 2004). A socialist religion, Islam is arguably a socialist religion with various teachings aimed at establishing economic equality (Hefner, 2017). (Naqvi, 1994/2013).

Those in the Nationalist Muslim (Class 2) and Moderate Muslim (Class 5) clusters tended to oppose Islamization in the public sphere. These two clusters differed in their strong attitudes toward issues of social inclusion. Nationalist Muslims generally take a neutral or normative stance on most issues. They followed more religious doctrines, which mainstream Indonesians value. Conversely, moderate Muslim participants were more likely to support socially inclusive issues. For example, they were more accepting of non-Muslim political leaders, more accepting of LGBT people, and more supportive of secular laws not based on their own religion. These two clusters shared similarity in many psychological traits (RWA, SDO, and NFCC) and Totalism in Islam scores. The Nationalist Muslim cluster had more females than the Moderate Muslim cluster. They also scored lower than the Fundamentalist Islam cluster in RWA, SDO, and NFCC.

The Progressive Muslim cluster (Class 6) was the most moderate stance in anti-Western attitudes (LGBT rights, closing of nightlife activities, and banning of alcohol). They tended to be more socialist on economic issues and less likely to support Islamization of the public sphere. However, compared to Nationalist and Moderate Muslims, Progressive Muslims scored higher on the Totalism in Islam scale. This finding reflects modern Muslims' acceptance of a globalist Muslim identity that seeks social justice for Muslim identities in various public domains (Kundnani, 2008). This is merely a hypothesis that should be investigated further. Compared to other classes, Progressive Muslims scored lower in RWA conventionalism. This may be because progressive liberal communities tend to be more supportive of social change and progress (Haidt, 2012; Osborne, Wootton, & Sibley, 2013). However, compared to Nationalist or Moderate Muslims, these people tend to be more egalitarian. Class 6 has the most egalitarian political attitudes of all the classes, so this result is puzzling. Although they oppose ultra-conservative groups (but not the secular status quo in Indonesia), it is possible that Class 6 has a more non-egalitarian orientation toward conservative groups. These people may be less likely to support fundamentalist social justice visions that seek to replace the secular status quo with Islam. Of course, this is all hypothetical, and future research should provide empirical evidence.

Individuals in the Apolitical Muslim cluster (Class 3) tend to be neutral or moderate on most issues. However, many of these people support the prohibition of alcohol and oppose LGBT rights. Remember that they are more moderate than Fundamentalist, Nationalist, and Moderate Muslims (higher frequency of neutral responses to the two issues). They ranked last in RWA's submission and aggression dimensions, along with Hijrah Muslims (Class 4). Apolitical Muslims may be less likely to be ideological because they are less concerned about all political issues (as evidenced by their mostly neutral attitudes) (Jerolmack & Porpora, 2004; Monroe & Kreidie, 1997). They also ranked second in SDO-D, the SDO dominance dimension. This result matched our explanation that Class 3 (Apolitical Muslim) tends to maintain the status quo in society and thus has less political attitudes.

Finally, the Hijrah Muslim cluster (Class 4) had more moderate support for socialist economic policies, but strong support for anti-Western policies and the establishment of Islamic influence in politics. To promote the Islamic identity, Hijrah communities sell exclusive and luxurious goods to promote the Islamic identity, and promote conservatism through the free market system (Amna, 2019; Hasan, 2019). We couldn't find much literature on this topic. This issue may be unique to Indonesia, where capitalism and Islam coexist (Hadiz, 2011). But, like the Fundamentalist Class, this Hijrah Class may express hatred towards perceived enemies (Mashuri & Zaduqisti, 2019). (Muluk, Sumaktoyo, & Ruth, 2013). The main difference is their acceptance of capitalism.

Across all classes, we found an overwhelming anti-Western influence on personal issues (banning alcohol, rejecting LGBT rights, closing nightclubs and bars). The rise of conservatism in Indonesia over the last decade may be blamed on these findings (Menchik & Trost, 2018; Mietzner & Muhtadi, 2018). Our study participants from the Fundamentalist Islam cluster (Class 1) showed this tendency. There were 462 Fundamentalist Muslims, far more than any other class. Despite the fact that this study showed that Muslims are very diverse, a third of the participants were puritanical and ultra-conservative.

As shown in Figure 2, although they tend to be neutral in attitudes towards supporting Islamic law, progressive Muslims are more supportive than moderate Muslims. This could be explained like this. In Indonesia, moderate Muslims often support traditional Indonesian values in opposition to sharia supporters (Robinson, 2011). On the other hand, progressive Muslims, drawn to Western liberal democracy, may not share traditional Indonesian values. But when it comes to other issues related to the Islamic law establishment, Progressives and Moderates have very similar views.

Other than providing novel insight regarding Muslim political camps, our findings revealed that fundamentalist Muslims who are more supportive of economic equality policies should be distinguished from Hijrah Muslims who welcome the free-market economy–even though both represent the face of religious fundamentalism. In addition, Muslims such as Nationalist and Moderate Muslims should be distinguished from Progressive Muslims. While the three groups seem to agree that secular laws (vs. sharia-based laws) should be maintained, these groups disagree on the issue of attitudes towards Western lifestyles.

This classification may help policymakers predict social movement narratives. For example, the political agenda underlying collective actions may vary between Muslim groups. Fundamentalists may use economic injustice and symbolic threat as narratives for their movement, whereas Hijrah Muslims may not. Meanwhile, progressives may fight for marginalized groups like the LGBT community. Nationalists may oppose this movement because they believe it threatens Indonesia's traditional family values.

In addition, we can map Indonesian political figures and parties based on the categories we established in our research. Such categorization may prove to be more meaningful in comparison to classifications based on a single political spectrum, such as liberal vs. conservative or modern vs traditional. With this in mind, we can predict which political candidates or parties will acquire support from each political camp.

We noted several limitations to this study. First, the latent class samples may not be sufficiently representative, as we only collected data from eight of Indonesia's 33 provinces. But we chose the eight provinces carefully, as they represent Indonesia's various Islamic traditions. Second, our sampling method was more convenience-based than systematic random sampling. This condition may jeopardize external validity. Third, we didn’t cover other relevant political issues like women's rights, freedom of expression, and solidarity with other Muslims (e.g., Uighur Muslims in China, Rohingya Muslims in Myanmar, Palestine Muslims). Inclusion of such issues may provide a broader picture of Muslim diversity. Future research should replicate this study with larger samples using a probabilistic method from all Indonesian provinces and focus on political issues. Fourth, we did not consider the correlates like intolerance and intergroup hostility. Future research should consider these variables to better understand and predict potential problematic groups that could cause conflict among all clusters.

Conclusion

We found six clusters of Muslim individuals based on political attitudes through latent class analysis: 1) Fundamentalist Muslim; 2) Nationalist Muslim; 3) Apolitical Muslim; 4) Hijrah Muslim; 5) Moderate Muslim; and 6) Progressive Muslim. These clusters differed in several psychological correlates such as Need for Cognitive Closure (NFCC), various dimensions of Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA), and various dimensions of Social Dominance Orientation (SDO). Therefore, we conclude that Muslim individuals cannot be categorized by a single spectrum or a dichotomic differentiation.

Funding

The authors have no funding to report.

Acknowledgments

The authors have no support to report.

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Supplementary Materials

The Supplementary Materials consist of (for access see Index of Supplementary Materials below):

  1. Supplementary Materials A: The details of frequencies for the latent class analysis. Within this document, we provided a more detailed number of the frequency in each issue

  2. Supplementary Materials B: The details of Lo, Mendell, and Rubin (LMR) test results which includes the degree of freedom and p-value for each class

  3. Supplementary Materials C: The post-hoc analyses with Tukey HSD which examined the differences of mean scores for Right-Wing Authoritarianism, Social Dominance Orientation, and Need for Cognitive Closure between classes

  4. Supplementary Materials D: The complete results of the latent profile analysis as an alternative to the latent class analysis (See the Method section)

Index of Supplementary Materials

  • Istiqomah, Hudiyana, J., Milla, M. N., Muluk, H., & Takwin, B. (2022). Supplementary materials to "Islam and politics: A latent class analysis of Indonesian Muslims based on political attitudes and psychological determinants" [Additional information]. PsychOpen GOLD. https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.5124

References

  • Albritton, R. B. (1999). Political diversity among Muslims in Thailand. Asian Studies Review, 23(2), 233-246. https://doi.org/10.1080/10357829908713233

  • Alford, J. R., Funk, C. L., & Hibbing, J. R. (2008). Beyond liberals and conservatives to political genotypes and phenotypes. Perspectives on Politics, 6(2), 321-328. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592708080638

  • Ali, M. (2005). The rise of the Liberal Islam Network (JIL) in contemporary Indonesia. The American Journal of Islamic Social Sciences, 22(1), 1-27. https://doi.org/10.35632/ajiss.v22i1.451

  • Altemeyer, B. (1996). The authoritarian specter. Cambridge, MA, USA: Harvard University Press.

  • Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. E. (1992). Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2(2), 113-133. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327582ijpr0202_5

  • Amna, A. (2019). Hijrah Artis Sebagai Komodifikasi Agama. Jurnal Sosiologi Reflektif, 13(2), 331-350. https://doi.org/10.14421/jsr.v13i12.1531

  • Aspinall, E., Fossati, D., Muhtadi, B., & Warburton, E. (2018, April 24). Mapping the Indonesian political spectrum. New Mandala, 5. https://www.newmandala.org/mapping-indonesian-political-spectrum/

  • Ayoob, M. (2009). The many faces of political Islam: Religion and politics in the Muslim world. Ann Arbor, MI, USA: University of Michigan Press.

  • Badan Pusat Statistik (Indonesian Central Bureau of Statistics). (2010). Penduduk menurut wilayah dan agama yang dianut [Population according to region and religion]. Retrieved from https://sp2010.bps.go.id/index.php/site/tabel?tid=321&wid=0

  • Badan Pusat Statistik (Indonesian Central Bureau of Statistics). (2020, July 15). Persentase Penduduk Miskin Maret 2020 naik menjadi 9,78 persen [The percentage of people below poverty line increased to 9,78 percent per March 2020]. Retrieved from https://www.bps.go.id/pressrelease/2020/07/15/1744/persentase-penduduk-miskin-maret-2020-naik-menjadi-9-78-persen.html

  • Barton, G. (2014). The Gülen movement, Muhammadiyah and Nahdlatul Ulama: Progressive Islamic thought, religious philanthropy and civil society in Turkey and Indonesia. Islam & Christian-Muslim Relations, 25(3), 287-301. https://doi.org/10.1080/09596410.2014.916124

  • Beaton, D. E., Bombardier, C., Guillemin, F., & Ferraz, M. B. (2000). Guidelines for the process of cross-cultural adaptation of self-report measures. Spine, 25(24), 3186-3191. https://doi.org/10.1097/00007632-200012150-00014

  • Beller, J., & Kröger, C. (2018). Religiosity, religious fundamentalism, and perceived threat as predictors of Muslim support for extremist violence. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 10(4), 345-355. https://doi.org/10.1037/rel0000138

  • Brandt, M. J., & Reyna, R. J. (2010). The role of prejudice and the need for closure in religious fundamentalism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(5), 715-725. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210366306

  • Branine, M., & Glover, I. (1997). Ageism in work and employment: Thinking about connections. Personnel Review, 26(4), 233-244. https://doi.org/10.1108/00483489710172042

  • Chua, A. (2004). World on fire: How exporting free market democracy breeds ethnic hatred and global instability. New York, NY, USA: Anchor Books.

  • De Zavala, A. G., Cislak, A., & Wesolowska, E. (2010). Political conservatism, need for cognitive closure, and intergroup hostility. Political Psychology, 31(4), 521-541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00767.x

  • Domingos, P. (1999). The role of Occam’s razor in knowledge discovery. Data Mining and Knowledge Discovery, 3(4), 409-425. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009868929893

  • Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual-process cognitive-motivational theory of ideology and prejudice. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 41-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(01)80004-6

  • Duckitt, J. (2006). Differential effects of right wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation on outgroup attitudes and their mediation by threat from and competitiveness to outgroups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(5), 684-696. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205284282

  • Duckitt, J., & Sibley, C. G. (2010). Personality, ideology, prejudice, and politics: A dual-process motivational model. Journal of Personality, 78(6), 1861-1894. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00672.x

  • Erikson, R. S., & Tedin, K. L. (2016). American public opinion (9th ed). New York, NY, USA: Routledge.

  • Enayat, H. (2005). Modern Islamic political thought: The response of the Shi ‘i and Sunni Muslims to the twentieth century. London, United Kingdom: Bloomsbury.

  • Feldman, S., & Johnston, C. (2014). Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychology, 35(3), 337-358. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12055

  • Friesen, J. P., Campbell, T. H., & Kay, A. C. (2015). The psychological advantage of unfalsifiability: The appeal of untestable religious and political ideologies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(3), 515-529. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000018

  • Fukuyama, F. (2006). The end of history and the last man. New York, NY, USA: Simon and Schuster.

  • Geertz, C. (1976). The religion of Java. Chicago, IL, USA: University of Chicago Press.

  • Hadiz, V. R. (2011). Indonesian political Islam: Capitalist development and the legacies of the Cold War. Journal of Current Southeast Asian Affairs, 30(1), 3-38. https://doi.org/10.1177/186810341103000101

  • Hadiz, V. R. (2018). Islamic populism and the politics of neoliberal inequalities. In C. De La Torre (Ed.), Routledge handbook of global populism (pp. 176-189). Abingdon, United Kingdom: Routledge.

  • Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York, NY, USA: Vintage.

  • Hasan, H. (2019). Contemporary religious movement in Indonesia: A study of Hijrah Festival in Jakarta in 2018. Journal of Indonesian Islam, 13(1), 230-265. https://doi.org/10.15642/JIIS.2019.13.1.230-265

  • Hefner, R. W. (2017). Ambivalent embrace: Islamic economics and global capitalism. In J. B. Imber (Ed.), Markets, morals, and religion (pp. 141-155). Routledge.

  • Hennes, E. P., Nam, H. H., Stern, C., & Jost, J. T. (2012). Not all ideologies are created equal: Epistemic, existential, and relational needs predict system-justifying attitudes. Social Cognition, 30(6), 669-688. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2012.30.6.669

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2-3), 61-83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X

  • Higgins, E. T., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press.

  • Ho, A. K., Sidanius, J., Pratto, F., Levin, S., Thomsen, L., Kteily, N., & Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2012). Social Dominance Orientation: Revisiting the structure and function of a variable predicting social and political attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(5), 583-606. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211432765

  • Huang, F. L. (2020). MANOVA: A procedure whose time has passed? Gifted Child Quarterly, 64(1), 56-60. https://doi.org/10.1177/0016986219887200

  • Huberty, C. J., & Morris, J. D. (1989). Multivariate analysis versus multiple univariate analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 105(2), 302-308. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.105.2.302

  • Huda, Q. U. (2006). The diversity of Muslims in the United States: Views as Americans (Vol. 31). Washington, DC, USA: United States Institute of Peace.

  • Hudiyana, J., Putra, I. E., Ariyanto, A. A., Brama, G. H., & Muluk, H. (2019). Can the activation of analytic cognitive style determine endorsement of secular belief? Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology, 13, Article E30. https://doi.org/10.1017/prp.2019.23

  • Huntington, S. P. (2000). The clash of civilizations? In L. Crothers & C. Lockhart (Eds.), Culture and politics (pp. 99-118). New York, NY, USA: Palgrave Macmillan.

  • Ibrahim, A. (2006). Universal values and Muslim democracy. Journal of Democracy, 17(3), 5-12. https://doi.org/10.1353/jod.2006.0046

  • Jackson, L. M., & Esses, V. M. (1997). Of scripture and ascription: The relation between religious fundamentalism and intergroup helping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(8), 893-906. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167297238009

  • Jerolmack, C., & Porpora, D. (2004). Religion, rationality, and experience: A response to the new rational choice theory of religion. Sociological Theory, 22(1), 140-160. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9558.2004.00208.x

  • Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. The American Psychologist, 61(7), 651-670. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.651

  • Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339-375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339

  • Jost, J. T., Napier, J. L., Thorisdottir, H., Gosling, S. D., Palfai, T. P., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Are needs to manage uncertainty and threat associated with political conservatism or ideological extremity? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(7), 989-1007. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207301028

  • Jost, J. T., Nosek, B. A., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Ideology: Its resurgence in social, personality, and political psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(2), 126-136. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00070.x

  • Kersten, C. (2015). Islam in Indonesia: The contest for society, ideas and values. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.

  • Kiley, J. (2017). In polarized era, fewer Americans hold a mix of conservative and liberal views. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2017/10/23/in-polarized-era-fewer-americans-hold-a-mix-of-conservative-and-liberal-views/

  • Kinder, D. R., & Kalmoe, N. P. (2017). Neither liberal nor conservative: Ideological innocence in the American public. Chicago, IL, USA: University of Chicago Press.

  • Koleva, S. P., Graham, J., Iyer, R., Ditto, P. H., & Haidt, J. (2012). Tracing the threads: How five moral concerns (especially Purity) help explain culture war attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(2), 184-194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.006

  • Kundnani, A. (2008). Islamism and the roots of liberal rage. Race & Class, 50(2), 40-68. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306396808096393

  • Lewis, G. J., & de-Wit, L. (2019). How many ways to say goodbye? The latent class structure and psychological correlates of European union sentiment in a large sample of UK adults. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 7(1), 556-576. https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v7i1.981

  • Liddle, R. W., & Mujani, S. (2007). Leadership, party, and religion: Explaining voting behavior in Indonesia. Comparative Political Studies, 40(7), 832-857. https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414006292113

  • Magidson, J., & Vermunt, J. K. (2002). Latent class model for clustering: A comparison with K-means. Canadian Journal of Marketing Research, 20(1), 37-44.

  • Mashuri, A., Akhrani, L. A., & Zaduqisti, E. (2016). You are the real terrorist and we are just your puppet: Using individual and group factors to explain Indonesian Muslims’ attributions of causes of terrorism. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 12(1), 68-98. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v12i1.1001

  • Mashuri, A., & Zaduqisti, E. (2019). Explaining Muslims’ aggressive tendencies towards the West: The role of negative stereotypes, anger, perceived conflict and Islamic fundamentalism. Psychology and Developing Societies, 31(1), 56-87. https://doi.org/10.1177/0971333618819151

  • Mearsheimer, J. J. (2001). The tragedy of great power politics. New York, NY, USA: WW Norton & Company.

  • Meleady, R., & Vermue, M. (2019). The effect of intergroup contact on solidarity‐based collective action is mediated by reductions in SDO. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 49(5), 307-318. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12586

  • Menchik, J. (2014). Productive intolerance: Godly nationalism in Indonesia. Comparative Studies in Society and History, 56(3), 591-621. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0010417514000267

  • Menchik, J. (2016). Islam and democracy in Indonesia: Tolerance without liberalism. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.

  • Menchik, J., & Trost, K. (2018). A” tolerant “Indonesia? Indonesian Muslims in comparative perspective. In R. Hefner (Ed.), Routledge handbook of contemporary Indonesia (pp. 390-405). Abingdon, United Kingdom: Routledge.

  • Mietzner, M., & Muhtadi, B. (2018). Explaining the 2016 Islamist mobilisation in Indonesia: Religious intolerance, militant groups and the politics of accommodation. Asian Studies Review, 42(3), 479-497. https://doi.org/10.1080/10357823.2018.1473335

  • Monroe, K. R., & Kreidie, L. H. (1997). The perspective of Islamic fundamentalists and the limits of rational choice theory. Political Psychology, 18(1), 19-43. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00043

  • Morgan, G. B. (2015). Mixed mode latent class analysis: An examination of fit index performance for classification. Structural Equation Modeling, 22(1), 76-86. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2014.935751

  • Mujani, S. (2007). Muslim demokrat: Islam, budaya demokrasi, dan partisipasi politik di Indonesia pasca Orde Baru. Jakarta, Indonesia: Gramedia Pustaka Utama.

  • Mujani, S. (2019). Explaining religio-political tolerance among Muslims: Evidence from Indonesia. Studia Islamika, 26(2), 319-351. https://doi.org/10.15408/sdi.v26i2.11237

  • Muluk, H., Hudiyana, J., & Shadiqi, M. A. (2018). The development of psychology of culture in Indonesia. In W. W. Li, D. Hodgetts, & K. H. Foo (Eds.), Asia-Pacific perspectives on intercultural psychology (pp. 140-156). Abingdon, United Kingdom: Routledge.

  • Muluk, H., & Sumaktoyo, N. G. (2010). Intratextual fundamentalism and the desire for simple cognitive structure: The moderating effect of the ability to achieve cognitive structure. Archiv für Religionspsychologie, 32(2), 217-238. https://doi.org/10.1163/157361210X500919

  • Muluk, H., Sumaktoyo, N. G., & Ruth, D. M. (2013). Jihad as justification: National survey evidence of belief in violent jihad as a mediating factor for sacred violence among Muslims in Indonesia. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 16(2), 101-111. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12002

  • Naqvi, S. N. H. (2013). Islam, economics, and society (RLE Politics of Islam). Abingdon, United Kingdom: Routledge. (Original work published 1994)

  • Nurdin, A. A. (2005). Islam and state: A study of the liberal Islamic network in Indonesia, 1999-2004. New Zealand Journal of Asian Studies, 7(2), 20-39.

  • Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(4), 535-569. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701575396

  • Osborne, D., Wootton, L. W., & Sibley, C. G. (2013). Are liberals agreeable or not? Politeness and compassion differentially predict political conservatism via distinct ideologies. Social Psychology, 44(5), 354-360. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000132

  • Passini, S. (2008). Exploring the multidimensional facets of authoritarianism: Authoritarian aggression and social dominance orientation. European Journal of Psychology Open, 67(1), 51-60. https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.67.1.51

  • Pepinsky, T. B., Liddle, R. W., & Mujani, S. (2012). Testing Islam’s political advantage: Evidence from Indonesia. American Journal of Political Science, 56(3), 584-600. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5907.2011.00570.x

  • Pepinsky, T. B., Liddle, R. W., & Mujani, S. (2018). Piety and public opinion: Understanding Indonesian Islam. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.

  • Pew Research Center. (2008, September 17). Religiosity. Retrieved from https://www.pewresearch.org/global/2008/09/17/chapter-2-religiosity/

  • Pituch, K. A., & Stevens, J. P. (2015). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences: Analyses with SAS and IBM’s SPSS. Abingdon, United Kingdom: Routledge.

  • Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(4), 741-763. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.741

  • Rad, M. S., Martingano, A. J., & Ginges, J. (2018). Toward a psychology of homo sapiens: Making psychological science more representative of the human population. Psychology and Cognitive Sciences: Open Journal, 115(45), 11401-11405. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721165115

  • Reese, G., Proch, J., & Cohrs, J. C. (2014). Individual differences in responses to global inequality. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 14, 217-238. https://doi.org/10.1111/asap.12032

  • Robinson, K. (2011). Sawerigading vs. sharia: Identities and political contestation in decentralised Indonesia. Asian Journal of Social Science, 39(2), 219-237. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853111X565896

  • Roets, A., & Van Hiel, A. V. (2011). Item selection and validation of a brief, 15-item version of the Need for Closure Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(1), 90-94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.004

  • Sakai, M., & Fauzia, A. (2014). Islamic orientations in contemporary Indonesia: Islamism on the rise? Asian Ethnicity, 15(1), 41-61. https://doi.org/10.1080/14631369.2013.784513

  • Shepard, W. E. (1987). Islam and ideology: Towards a typology. International Journal of Middle East Studies, 19(3), 307-336. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0020743800056750

  • Skitka, L. J., & Tetlock, P. E. (1993). Providing public assistance: Cognitive and motivational processes underlying liberal and conservative policy preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(6), 1205-1223. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1205

  • Slater, D., & Arugay, A. A. (2018). Polarizing figures: Executive power and institutional conflict in Asian democracies. The American Behavioral Scientist, 62(1), 92-106. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764218759577

  • Stark, R. (1999). Secularization R.I.P. Sociology of Religion, 60(3), 249-273. https://doi.org/10.2307/3711936

  • Stewart, A. L., & Tran, J. (2018). Protesting racial hierarchy: Testing a social dominance theory model of collective action among white Americans. The Journal of Social Issues, 74(2), 299-316. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12270

  • Tomsa, D. (2019). Islamism and party politics in Indonesia. In P. Djupe, M. J. Rozell, & T. Jelen (Eds.), Oxford research encyclopaedia of politics. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.013.1157

  • van Bruinessen, M. (1999). Global and local in Indonesian Islam. Southeast Asian Studies, 37(2), 158-175.

  • WageIndicator.org. (2021). Minimum Wage – Indonesia. Retrieved from https://wageindicator.org/salary/minimum-wage/indonesia

  • Wieringa, S. E. (2015). Gender harmony and the happy family: Islam, gender and sexuality in post-Reformasi Indonesia. South East Asia Research, 23(1), 27-44. https://doi.org/10.5367/sear.2015.0244

  • Williams, G. A., & Kibowski, F. (2015). Latent class analysis and latent profile analysis. In L. A. Jason & D. Glenwick (Eds.), Handbook of methodological approaches to community-based research: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods (pp. 143-152). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.

  • Williamson, W. P., & Hood, R. W., Jr. (2014). Religious fundamentalism and perceived threat: A report from an experimental study. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 17(5), 520-528. https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2013.857297

  • Zubaida, S. (2005). Islam and secularization. Asian Journal of Social Science, 33(3), 438-448. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853105775013607