Original Research Reports

Self-Censorship Orientation: Scale Development, Correlates and Outcomes

Keren Sharvit*a, Daniel Bar-Talb, Boaz Hameiricd, Anat Zafrane, Eldad Shaharc, Amiram Ravivcf

Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 2018, Vol. 6(2), 331–363, https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v6i2.859

Received: 2017-09-11. Accepted: 2018-06-19. Published (VoR): 2018-07-27.

Handling Editor: Johanna Vollhardt, Clark University, Worcester, MA, USA

*Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of Haifa, 199 Aba Khoushy Ave., Haifa 3498838, Israel. E-mail: ksharvit@psy.haifa.ac.il

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Self-censorship is defined as intentionally and voluntarily withholding information from others in absence of formal obstacles. We conducted cross-sectional and longitudinal research to develop a quantitative measure of individuals’ Self-Censorship Orientation (SCO) and investigated its correlates and outcomes in the context of the intractable Israeli-Palestinian conflict. Stage 1 investigated the factor structure of the scale and its convergent and discriminant validity in a representative sample (N = 499). Findings revealed two negatively related factors representing preferences for self-censorship and for disclosure of information. The factors were distinct from measures of similar constructs and correlated as expected with variables representing conservatism, ingroup commitment and universalistic values. In Stage 2, participants were re-surveyed five months later to establish test-retest reliability and predictive validity. SCO factors assessed at Stage 1 predicted readiness to conceal or reveal information portraying the ingroup’s conduct in the conflict negatively beyond all Stage 1 measures. The SCO scale provides a reliable and valid instrument for future investigations of self-censorship and its individual and societal implications.

Keywords: self-censorship, censorship, disclosure, information, freedom of expression

תקציר

צנזורה עצמית מוגדרת כהסתרה רצונית ומכוונת של מידע מפני אחרים בהעדר איסורים או חסמים רשמיים. ביצענו מחקר רוחב ואורך בכדי לפתח מדד כמותני לנטיה אישית לצנזורה עצמית, ובחנו משתנים שנטיה זו קשורה אליהם ואת תוצאותיה בהקשר של הסכסוך הבלתי נשלט בין ישראלים לפלסטינים. בשלב הראשון בחנו את מבנה הגורמים של המדד ואת התוקף המתכנס והמבחין שלו בקרב מדגם מייצג (N = 499). הממצאים חשפו שני גורמים הנמצאים במתאם שלילי זה עם זה, שייצגו העדפות לצנזורה עצמית ולחשיפה של מידע. הגורמים היו מובחנים ממדדים של מושגים דומים והיו קשורים כמצופה עם מדדים המייצגים שמרנות, מחויבות לקבוצת הפנים וערכים אוניברסליים. בשלב השני המשיבים נסקרו שוב לאחר חמישה חודשים בכדי לבסס מהימנות מבחן חוזר ותוקף ניבוי. גורמי המדד שנמדדו בשלב הראשון ניבאו את הנכונות להסתיר או לחשוף מידע המציג את התנהלות קבוצת הפנים בסכסוך באור שלילי, וזאת מעל ומעבר למדדים אחרים מהשלב הראשון. המדד שפותח מספק כלי מהימן ותקף למחקרים עתידיים בנושא צנזורה עצמית והשלכותיה ברמת הפרט והחברה.

מילות מפתח: צנזורה עצמית, צנזורה, חשיפת מידע, מידע, חופש ביטוי

Non-Technical Summary

Background

Self-censorship is an intentional voluntary choice to withhold information from others, even when no formal obstacles prevent the dissemination of this information.

Why was this study done?

The research aimed to develop a questionnaire that may assess people’s tendency to self-censor, or Self-Censorship Orientation (SCO). We were interested in individual characteristics that may be related to SCO and the outcomes that this orientation might predict in a context of protracted inter-group conflict, particularly the Israeli-Palestinian conflict.

What did the researchers do and find?

In the first stage of the research, we administered the measure of SCO and measures of other relevant individual characteristics to a representative sample of Jewish Israelis. The findings revealed that the SCO scale can assess two dimensions: the tendency to self-censor and the tendency to disclose information. The scores on these dimensions were related to conservatism, commitment to one’s group and universalistic values. In the second stage of the study, participants were re-surveyed five months after the first stage, and were presented with information that portrayed their group’s conduct in the conflict negatively. SCO scores from the first stage were able to predict the readiness to conceal or reveal this information.

What do these findings mean?

The findings suggest that the tendency to self-censor or disclose information can predict meaningful future outcomes. The scale developed in this research can be useful in future studies of self-censorship and its implications.

What does a person do when he or she knows of wrongdoings carried out within a group to which he or she is committed? Usually the person faces a dilemma: Revealing the information can prevent or correct the wrongs and help the group improve, but it will also reflect negatively on the group. In many cases, individuals choose to keep information to themselves, even when they are certain of the information’s validity and even when there are no formal obstacles such as censorship that prevent its exposure. We refer to this phenomenon as self-censorship. Due to a variety of reasons, individuals may decide not to reveal truthful information to their family members, close friends, fellow group members, social leaders, or other society members. In all of these cases they believe that there is a cost to revealing the information and therefore choose to withhold it.

Self-censorship has been noted by behavioral and social scientists in various domains. In the context of family relations it is discussed extensively as secret keeping (Brown-Smith, 1998; Karpel, 1980; Petronio, 2010; Vangelisti, 1994). Self-censorship has also been noted in the context of organizations, where it is known as silence (Morrison & Milliken, 2000; Van Dyne, Ang, & Botero, 2003) sometimes broken by whistle blowers (Gundlach, Douglas, & Martinko, 2003; Henik, 2015; Near & Miceli, 1996; Westin, 1981). Self-censorship is also observed quite frequently in societal institutions such as mass media or societal-cultural agencies (Antilla, 2010; Kenny & Gross, 2008; Lee & Chan, 2009; Nelkin, 1995).

For example, Maksudyan (2009) analyzed translations of history books about World War I into Turkish in Turkey. She found that writers, translators and editors censored themselves when addressing information regarding what is considered by most of the world as the Armenian genocide during this period. They did so in order to avoid public censorship and/or in order to achieve approval from the formal institutions. As another example, Antilla (2010) noted that the news media in the United States practice self-censorship in reporting scientific findings about the effects of human actions on global warming.

In this article we focus on self-censorship in societal contexts. We address cases in which it is clear that the self-censored information may contribute to improved societal functioning and/or prevent wrongdoings within the society. Nonetheless, individuals withhold the information from other society members and outsiders, which can affect the well-being of societies and their members. One example of such a context is intergroup conflict, which is the focus of our research and is discussed elaborately in what follows.

Individuals may differ in their inclination to practice self-censorship. Therefore, it is of considerable interest to assess individuals’ propensity toward self-censorship. The purpose of the present research was to develop an instrument to assess individuals’ Self-Censorship Orientation (SCO) and to situate this construct among other established psychological constructs. Toward this aim, we examine the correlates of SCO and its consequences.

The Concept of Self-Censorship

We define self-censorship of information as intentionally and voluntarily withholding information from others in the absence of formal obstacles (Bar-Tal, 2017). Self-censorship typically occurs in situations in which the actor possesses new information that is unknown to others, believes that this information is valid, and believes that it has implications for others. By self-censoring, the individual prevents others’ free access to the information and informally regulates the flow of information in society. Self-censorship then becomes a barrier to the dissemination of information that may be useful to the group. When individuals self-censor extensively, the functioning of democratic societies may be disrupted (for an extensive discussion see Bar-Tal, Nets-Zehngut, & Sharvit, 2017).

The present conception of self-censorship focuses on withholding information rather than opinions. Information is viewed as factual knowledge that can be based on personal experience, reports from others, research or documents. It refers to something that actually happened and that is considered verified and valid.i Of importance is individuals’ belief that the information is based on evidence and does not depend on personal views. Opinions are considered as going beyond the evidence and reflecting subjective inferences, attributions, evaluations and impressions. Their expression is excluded from our definition of self-censorship. Legal scholars make a similar distinction between information and opinions (Barendt, 2005; Schauer, 1982). The emphasis on information distinguishes our conception of self-censorship from previous conceptions. Specifically, Hayes, Glynn, and Shanahan (2005a, p. 298, 2005b, p. 443) define self-censorship as “withholding of one's true opinion from an audience perceived to disagree with that opinion” and maintain that conformity is a form of self-censorship. In our view, focusing on opinions rather than information does not allow sufficient distinction between the concept of self-censorship and the concept of conformity, which we discuss extensively in the following section.

Additionally, our definition suggests that self-censorship is practiced only when there are no formal obstacles to revealing information and the decision to withhold information is made voluntarily. The definition therefore excludes cases in which there is an institutionalized external obstacle that prevents dissemination of information, such as formal censorship (e.g., De Baets, 2002; Jansen, 1988; Tribe, 1973).

A number of different but not mutually exclusive motivations may drive individuals to self-censor (see Nets-Zehngut, Pliskin, & Bar-Tal, 2015). A salient motivation is protection of the ingroup based on the assumption that the information may hurt the group or its cause if revealed. More specifically, Bar-Tal (2017) suggests that self-censorship often reflects resolution of a dilemma between conflicting goals or needs. On the one hand, individuals are motivated to share information with others. In addition to the pro-social function of providing important information to others, sharing of information contributes to social coordination, to forming a meaningful understanding of events, and to relief of unpleasant emotions. On the other hand, individuals often identify strongly with social groups, which become sources from which they derive part of their self-concept (Tajfel, 1978). Moreover, in some cases individuals feel that they share a common fate with the group and experience concern for the welfare of the collective (David & Bar-Tal, 2009). Consequently, they are motivated to maintain a positive image of the group and to prevent harm to the group. Hence, when individuals are in possession of information that may have negative implications for a group with which they identify, they experience a dilemma between the need to share the information and their motivation to protect the group. In some cases, they may decide to resolve the dilemma by self-censoring the information.

Societies often employ social sanctions against members who disseminate information that may be damaging to the group and reward members who display loyalty. This can encourage group members to self-censor in order to protect their own self-interest as well as that of the group. In other cases, however, concern for the group may lead individuals to reveal information even if it is damaging to the group's image, as a way of improving the group's functioning (Packer, 2008).

Self-Censorship Versus Conformity

Self-censorship must be distinguished from the related concept of conformity. Conformity is the tendency to change one's beliefs, attitudes, or behaviors to match those of the majority of others (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004). This change is in response to pressure by group members, which can be concrete (e.g., expression by others of what should be said) or imagined (e.g., based on past experiences). The literature differentiates between private conformity (also acceptance or conversion) and public conformity (also compliance). The former takes place when individuals truly accept the beliefs, attitudes or behaviors of the majority viewing them as truthful. The latter takes place when individuals express the views of the majority publically, but continue to hold different views privately. Individuals may conform because they do not want to appear deviant (normative conformity), or because they want to have accurate and valid knowledge (informational conformity; Deutsch & Gerard, 1955).

Several features distinguish conformity from our conception of self-censorship. First, conformity can involve either expressing or withholding opinions or information, whereas the present conception of self-censorship focuses only on withholding information. Second, individuals who conform believe that they are in the minority and withhold their beliefs either because they question their validity or because they are concerned about negative reactions by the majority. Such concern is also central to the conception of self-censorship proposed by Hayes et al. (2005a, 2005b), who found that individuals high (versus low) on their measure of self-censorship are more sensitive to the opinion climate of a group when deciding whether to express their opinions (Hayes, Uldall, & Glynn, 2010). However, in our conception of self-censorship individuals may withhold information when they are in the majority, if they believe that revealing the information could hurt their group. More generally, in cases of conformity the primary motivation is to protect the self, while in the case of self-censorship, according to the present conception, a central motivation is often to protect the group.

Self-Censorship in a Context of Intergroup Conflict

Bar-Tal (2017) proposed that the likelihood of self-censorship can be a function of individual differences, as well as the social context and specific circumstances. In the present research, we examine individual differences in the readiness to self-censor and their implications in the specific context of an intractable intergroup conflict. Studies suggest that the context of intractable conflict is especially conducive to self-censorship (Bar-Tal et al., 2017; Hameiri, Sharvit, Bar-Tal, Shahar, & Halperin, 2017). Societies involved in intractable conflict develop conflict-supporting narratives, which serve important functions when facing violence and war. These functions include justification of the group’s behavior, mobilization of society members to participate in the conflict and maintaining a positive group identity (Bar-Tal, 2013). Dissemination of information that contradicts these conflict-supporting narratives is discouraged because it is seen as detrimental to the group’s cause in the conflict (Bar-Tal, Oren, & Nets-Zehngut, 2014). In this context, self-censorship is often viewed by authorities and segments of society as necessary in order to protect the ingroup and its interests, leading to the development of norms that encourage self-censorship.

Hameiri, Sharvit, et al. (2017) conducted a longitudinal study to investigate antecedents and consequences of support for self-censorship during a violent confrontation in the context of the intractable Israeli-Palestinian conflict. The study included a measure referring to free flow of information in the media (reverse-scored) as well as a specific measure referring to support for self-censorship regarding harmful actions by Israelis toward Palestinians in the course of a violent confrontation (Operation Pillar of Defense; OPD). Notably, the measures in this study referred to support for self-censorship by others rather than respondents’ willingness to self-censor themselves. The findings showed that support for self-censorship during OPD was predicted by Right Wing Authoritarianism (RWA), ethnocentrism, siege mentality, and political orientation assessed prior to the operation, and was negatively related to support for conciliatory measures assessed after the operation. This suggested that support for self-censorship during violent confrontation can become a barrier to conflict resolution (see also Hameiri, Bar-Tal, & Halperin, 2017).

Additional studies examining self-censorship in the context of a violent intergroup conflict were conducted by Shahar, Hameiri, Bar-Tal, and Raviv (2018). In these studies, Israeli-Jewish participants were presented with hypothetical scenarios in which they might receive information about harmful conduct by members of their own group toward Palestinians, and were asked to indicate to whom they would be willing to reveal the information. There was no assessment of participants’ general orientation toward self-censorship independent of content and role. The findings showed that participants were more willing to disclose the information to individuals who were socially close to them than to more distant persons, particularly outgroup members. In addition, the second study found that participants were more willing to self-censor the information when they were hypothetically assigned to the role of a soldier than the role of civilian, and more willing to self-censor when the perpetrators of the harmful acts were soldiers rather than civilians. The present research aims to extend past findings by examining the implications of individual differences in a general orientation toward self-censorship, regardless of a specific outlet, topic or role, for the willingness to withhold or reveal information related to a violent intergroup conflict.

The Present Research

Measuring a general orientation toward self-censorship behavior is a challenge, because not everyone has necessarily experienced situations in which they needed to decide whether to disseminate or withhold information, and it is difficult to create such situations artificially. Therefore, we decided to focus on assessing the perceived appropriateness of self-censorship and the readiness to do so in certain situations. Our underlying assumption is that individuals who support the use of self-censorship are also more likely to practice it. The work of Hayes et al. (2005a, 2005b) on self-censorship similarly focused on the willingness to self-censor (WTSC), though as mentioned, their conception of self-censorship is different from ours.

The construction of the scale involved two stages. In the first stage we created a measure of SCO and collected data from a representative population sample in order to investigate the factor structure of the scale, determine its final item composition and test its correlates and implications in a context of violent intractable conflict. The second stage took place five months after the first. In that stage we re-surveyed as many of the participants from the first stage as possible in order to investigate the test-retest reliability and predictive validity of the scale, again in the context of an intractable conflict.

Stage 1: Developing the Self-Censorship Orientation (SCO) Scale

The scale construction process at Stage 1 was theoretically driven and based on the definition of self-censorship provided earlier. With respect to correlates, we first looked at the WTSC scale (Hayes et al., 2005a). As mentioned earlier, the WTSC assesses willingness to withhold one’s opinions, whereas our measure of SCO concerns the readiness to withhold valid information. Accordingly, we expected our measure of SCO to be distinct from WTSC. Nevertheless, because some of the factors underlying the willingness to withhold opinions and the willingness to withhold information may be similar, we expected the two measures to be positively correlated.

In establishing the discriminant validity of the WTSC, Hayes et al. (2005a) demonstrated that is was related to, but distinct from, several other indicators including argumentativeness (Infante & Rancer, 1982) and fear of negative evaluation (Leary, 1983). The former refers to individuals’ tendency to seek out and enjoy arguments, and the latter to experiencing distress in situations in which one is evaluated negatively. We expected our measure of SCO to also be distinct yet related to these measures. The expectation of a positive correlation between self-censorship and fear of negative evaluation is consistent with previous findings showing that whistle-blowing and breaking silence behaviors are associated with low self-monitoring (Jos, Tompkins, & Hays, 1989; Premeaux & Bedeian, 2003), which is a tendency to be guided by situational cues of social appropriateness (Snyder, 1974). Other measures used by Hayes et al. (2005a), such as shyness and self-esteem, were considered less relevant to our conception of SCO and therefore not included.

Our conception suggests that a major driving force behind SCO is a desire to protect one's ingroup, including its image, its norms, and its dominant narratives. Accordingly, we expected SCO to be positively related to indicators of commitment to the group, such as group identification and blind patriotism (Schatz, Staub, & Lavine, 1999), as well as indicators of adherence to group norms and narratives, such as conformity and firmly entrenched narrative closure (FENCE; Klar & Baram, 2016). Blind patriotism is defined as: "an attachment to country characterized by unquestioning positive evaluation, staunch allegiance, and intolerance of criticism” (Schatz et al., 1999, p. 151). FENCE was proposed as an individual-difference construct characterizing motivations to protect the historical group narrative (Klar & Baram, 2016). The desire to protect the ingroup, in turn, should increase with increased concern for the group's safety. Hence, we expected SCO to be positively related to indicators of individual fear and collective angst (Wohl, Branscombe, & Reysen, 2010). The association of self-censorship with fear and angst is also consistent with previous research in the organizational context indicating that silence at work is related to neuroticism (Nikolaou, Vakola, & Bourantas, 2008).

Right-wing authoritarianism (RWA; Altemeyer, 1996) refers to a personality type which advocates a conservative view of the world, including adherence to traditional values and closure to new ideas. RWA is associated with adherence to convention and conformity (Feldman, 2003) and with perception of the world as dangerous (Duckitt & Sibley, 2009). In the organizational context, it has been suggested that authoritarianism may be associated with employee silence (Timming & Johnstone, 2015). Thus, we expected SCO to be positively related to RWA as well.

Finally, we included measures of universalistic values, support for abstract democratic values, and constructive patriotism. Universalistic values focus on the understanding, appreciation, tolerance, and protection of all people, as well as equality, social justice, broadmindedness, and world peace (Schwartz, 2007). Abstract democratic values include freedom of expression, equality, and justice (Shamir & Sullivan, 1985). Constructive patriotism refers to individuals’ emotional affinity to a group accompanied by a motivation to resist the group's actions when they betray fundamental values (Schatz et al., 1999). We expected SCO to decrease when commitment to values other than group protection increases. Accordingly, we expected SCO to be negatively related to support for universal values, abstract democratic values and constructive patriotism. A negative relationship between self-censorship and general values is consistent with research in the organizational domain indicating that voice and whistle-blowing are associated with strong commitment to moral rules, as well as duty and conscientiousness (Jos et al., 1989; Nikolaou et al., 2008; Tangirala, Kamdar, Venkataramani, & Parke, 2013)

An additional aim of Stage 1 was to conduct a preliminary test of the predictive power of SCO in the context of a violent intractable conflict (which was explored further in Stage 2). We expected that individuals high on SCO would be less willing to reveal to outsiders specific information that portrays their ingroup’s conduct in the conflict negatively and the rival group’s behavior positively, compared to information that portrays the ingroup positively and the outgroup negatively.

Method

Sampling and Procedure

We administered an online survey to a nationwide sample of 499 Israeli Jewish participants between the ages of 18 and 64. The surveys were distributed by the research firm Midgam Project (MP) and participants were randomly selected from Midgam's opt-in panel. Participants who completed the survey had the following socio-demographic characteristics: Mean age 39.00 years (SD = 13.58); 51% female; 51% considered themselves secular, 29% were observant, 15% were religious and 6% were Ultra-Orthodox; 26% had at least a college degree; 39% had some college or professional education, and 35% had 12 or fewer years of schooling; 57% regarded themselves as having a rightist political orientation; 24% regarded themselves as centrists, and 18% regarded themselves as leftists. The sample's socio-demographic characteristics largely represented the distribution in the relevant population (Central Bureau of Statistics, 2013).

Measures

Willingness to reveal specific information about ingroup and outgroup behaviors in an intergroup conflict

This measure was administered at the beginning of the survey in order to ensure that participants’ responses are not affected by the assessment of SCO or any other variables. We informed participants that one purpose of the study was to assist the EU in creating a website with information about conflicts around the world, including the Israeli-Palestinian conflict. We selected the EU as the website creator for two reasons. On the one hand, it is a positive reference group for Israelis. Israel has numerous agreements with the EU in different domains and aspires to become an EU member. On the other hand, the EU is often critical of Israel’s policies and actions regarding the occupied Palestinian territories. Therefore, Israelis are likely motivated to influence the views of EU members on the Israeli-Palestinian conflict and may apply self-censorship in service of this goal.

We presented the participants with eight images and asked them to indicate the extent to which each image portrayed a typical situation in the Israeli-Palestinian conflict, and the extent to which it was appropriate for the website (1 = “not at all” and 6 = “absolutely”). Half of the images portrayed Palestinians as aggressors and/or Israelis as victims (consistent with the dominant Israeli-Jewish narrative), and the rest portrayed Israelis as aggressors and/or Palestinians as victims (contradicting the dominant narrative) (see also Shahar et al., 2018). We computed an index of appropriateness of narrative-consistent images (α = .85) and an index of appropriateness of narrative contradicting images (α = .89).

The self-censorship orientation scale (SCO)

In order to construct the scale, we began by formulating a mapping sentence describing the presumed characteristics of individuals who are high or low on SCO as derived from the theoretical definition of self-censorship proposed by Bar-Tal (2017) and reviewed earlier (see Table 1).

On the basis of the mapping sentence, the six authors generated through discussion 51 statements referring to beliefs, attitudes, emotions, motivations and behaviors reflecting high and low SCO. Next, we asked eight judges, who had previously conducted research on self-censorship, to review the statements and indicate the extent to which they reflected components of the mapping sentence. The judges noted that some statements were inconsistent with the theoretical reasoning, and others were unclear or could be interpreted in different ways. Following the judges' comments, we discarded some statements, changed the wording of others, and added new items. We then asked the judges to review the items again and repeated the process, ending with 31 statements that were selected for administration to participants.

Table 1

Mapping Sentence of Self-Censorship Orientation (SCO)

A person high in SCO will… A person low in SCO will…
Support censorship of information that might challenge his/her worldview and/or portray his/her ingroup negatively; Oppose censorship of information even if it might challenge his/her worldview and/or portray his/her ingroup negatively;
Do so on the basis of values and motivations of loyalty to the group, group protection and maintenance of group unity, as well as loyalty to personal beliefs and a desire to protect them; Do so on the basis of universal values and motivations, including freedom of expression and information, believing that disclosing the information would contribute to the group;
Respond with negative emotions to the disclosure of such information; Respond with positive emotions to the disclosure of such information;
Perceive the information as not representative of the group, potentially harmful to it, or challenging the individual’s worldview; Perceive the information as representative of the group, even if it challenges the individual’s worldview, and believe that concealing it would be harmful to the group;
Form negative judgements of others who disclose such information; Form positive judgements of others who disclose such information to various bodies;
If coming across such information, will not disclose it and will act to conceal it; If coming across such information, will be motivated to disclose it;
Support sanctions against persons or bodies that disclose such information Oppose sanctions against persons or bodies that disclose such information

Many of the statements referred to a group to which respondents belong. Because individuals may identify with numerous groups, we asked participants, before responding to the statements, to specify the group with which they most identify out of several options (religious group, state, nation, ethnic group, workplace, local community, or political party/organization). Participants were then instructed to respond to all subsequent statements in reference to the group specified using a 6-point scale ranging between 1 = "not at all" and 6 = "to a great extent." The same rating scale was used for all the following measures as well.

Willingness to self-censor (WTSC)

This scale, developed by Hayes et al. (2005a), refers to the tendency to withhold one's opinions. The scale includes eight items (e.g., "It is difficult for me to express my opinion if I think others won’t agree with what I say") with α = .81.

Argumentativeness

We used an abbreviated version of the scale developed by Infante and Rancer (1982) comprised of seven items (α = .60; sample item: "Arguing over controversial issues improves my intelligence").

Fear of negative evaluation

We used nine items (α = .86) from the short form of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (Leary, 1983). A sample item is "I worry about what other people will think of me even when I know it doesn't make any difference".

Group identification

We assessed identification in two ways, using measures created for the present research. First, participants rated, in reference to the group to which they are most attached (as indicated earlier), the extent to which belonging to this group was important to them, and the extent to which they were emotionally attached to the group (α = .87). This measure could refer to different groups for different participants. In addition, because the outcome measures in the present study addressed the context of the Israeli-Palestinian conflict, participants’ identification as Jewish Israelis was of particular interest. In order to assess it, participants responded to four items (α = .92) referring to their Jewish Israeli identification specifically (e.g., "How important to you is belonging to the Jewish nation?").

Patriotism

We used three blind patriotism items (α = .68; e.g., "I support Israel's policies for the very reason that they are the policies of my country") and three constructive patriotism items (α = .65; e.g., "I express my love for Israel by supporting efforts at positive change") from the measure developed by Schatz et al. (1999).

Conformity

This scale was adapted from the Social Conformity versus Autonomy scale (Feldman, 2003). The original scale items are comprised of pairs of statements, one representing a preference for individual autonomy and one for social conformity. For the present study, we selected eight autonomy statements (e.g., "People can only develop their true potential in a fully permissive society") and six conformity statements (e.g., "Obeying the rules and fitting in are signs of a strong and healthy society"), and asked the participants to rate their agreement with each statement. After reverse scoring the autonomy items, the scale yielded acceptable internal consistency (α = .77).

Firmly entrenched narrative closure (FENCE)

This scale assesses the value that individuals place on protecting their group's historical narrative in the context of an intergroup conflict (Klar & Baram, 2016). FENCE was found to be negatively related to openness to counter-narratives and quality of recall of information related to intergroup conflict. In the present study we used five items from the FENCE scale (α = .65; sample item: "Having many opinions about the conflict weakens us in the face of our enemies").

Fear

We used six items (α = .83) from the Positive and Negative Affectivity Schedule (PANAS-X; Watson & Clark, 1999) referring to experiences of fear in general (e.g., afraid, scared).

Collective angst

We used three items (α = .69) from the scale developed by Wohl and Branscombe (2009). A sample item is: "I feel anxious about the future of our state and society".

Right Wing Authoritarianism (RWA)

We used a 7-item abbreviated version (α = .79) of Altemeyer's (1996) original RWA scale. A sample item is "Obedience and respect for authority are the most important virtues children can learn". Similar abbreviated versions of the RWA scale were used in previous studies (see Feldman & Stenner, 1997; Halperin & Bar-Tal, 2011; Huddy, Feldman, & Cassese, 2007).

Universalistic values

Participants completed the universalism sub-scale from the Schwartz (1992, 2007) Value Survey, which includes four items (α = .82).

Support for abstract democratic values

Participants responded to three items (α = .74) from Shamir (1991). A sample item is "No matter what a person’s political beliefs are, they are entitled to the same legal rights as anyone else".

Results

Factorial Validity and Reliability of the SCO Scale

To test the factorial validity of the scale, participants were randomly divided into two groups. The first group was used to derive a preliminary version of the scale by means of exploratory factor analysis (EFA), and the second group was used to confirm this version of the scale using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). A first EFA was thus conducted with the 31 items in a random sample of 250 participants using maximum likelihood and oblimin rotation. In a simulation study by MacCallum, Widaman, Zhang, and Hong (1999), a sample size of 200 yielded convergent and admissible solutions in EFA in at least 95% of the cases where the item to factor ratio was 10:3, and in 100% of the cases where the ratio was 20:3. Hence, our sample size of 250 had a very high likelihood of yielding an admissible solution for an item to factor ratio greater than 10:3 (or more than 3 items per factor).

The EFA yielded four factors accounting for 53% of the variance. However, the fourth factor included only one item, which cross-loaded on the first factor as well. Hence, we decided to discard this item. Next, we proceeded to eliminate items with cross-loadings, as well as those with weak loadings below .30 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989). This left only two items in the third factor, which had originally included three items. The common element of the two remaining items seemed to be a similar grammatical structure rather than a meaningful theoretical construct. Therefore, we decided to eliminate all items loading on the third factor. At the end of the elimination process, we were left with 14 items loading on two factors. In the first factor, labeled "self-censorship", we retained six items referring to a readiness to conceal valid information that portrays the group negatively and contradicts one's beliefs. In the second factor, labeled "disclosure", we retained eight items referring to a willingness to disclose valid information about problematic group conduct.

A second EFA (with maximum likelihood and oblimin rotation) was then conducted with the 14 items that remained after the exclusion process based on the results of the original EFA. Results revealed a two-factor solution with eigenvalues of 5.62 and 1.39 representing 40.11% and 9.95% of the variance, respectively. The oblimin factor rotation revealed that all the items referring to withholding information loaded strongly on the "self-censorship" factor, and all the items referring to disclosure of information loaded strongly on the "disclosure" factor (all loadings > .50). No cross-loadings greater than .30 were observed. Table 2 shows descriptive statistics of the retained items and the results of the second EFA.

Table 2

Descriptive Statistics of Items in the SCO Scale and Results of an Exploratory Factor Analysis

Factor Loadings
M SD Disclosure factor Self-censorship factor
1. If I would encounter problematic conduct among my group members, I would feel responsible to bring that information to light. 3.97 1.18 .814 .037
2. It is important to expose letdowns by group members in order to learn from them and improve. 4.18 1.23 .745 .000
3. Anyone who knows about wrongdoings in our group and chooses to expose them is revealing real concern for the group. 3.89 1.37 .720 -.034
4. We ought to support members of our group who exposed wrongdoings among us. 4.07 1.51 .649 .103
5. I am angered to hear about group members who conceal credible information about wrongdoings in the group in order to protect its reputation. 3.52 1.36 .646 -.079
6. I am concerned that concealing credible information about wrongdoings in my group will lead us to moral decline. 3.76 1.49 .641 -.002
7. It is okay to expose wrongdoings by my group members, even if it causes disagreements within the group. 3.88 1.27 .638 -.069
8. It is wrong to conceal information about problematic conduct by my group members, because that usually represents a broader phenomenon. 3.50 1.36 .512 -.145
9. People who disclose credible information to external sources, which exposes my group to criticism, should be condemned. 3.31 1.52 .059 .817
10. Exposing credible information which presents our group in a negative light is playing into the hands of our enemies. 4.36 1.48 .067 .793
11. Providing credible information which presents our group in a negative light to the media weakens the group’s ability to withstand. 3.87 1.46 .052 .780
12. Although freedom of speech is an important value, people ought to restrict themselves in revealing information which may harm the group’s reputation, even if that information is credible. 3.52 1.41 -.211 .708
13. It is legitimate for people to withhold information which goes against their world-view, even if that information is credible. 3.43 1.46 -.082 .564
14. Some social institutions should not be opened to public criticism. 3.09 1.71 -.094 .538

Note. Bold-face indicates highest factor loadings.

The results of the EFA appear to suggest the presence of two factors in the SCO scale. However, previous research has shown that factor loadings of this sort may be an artifact of item-wording (Biderman, Nguyen, Cunningham, & Ghorbani, 2011; Marsh, Scalas, & Nagengast, 2010). In order to test whether the two factors obtained in the EFA reflect different measurement methods or substantive content differences, we conducted confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) with AMOS 23 software to model a multi-trait multi-method matrix (Bagozzi & Yi, 1990) using the second random sample of 249 participants. We tested the fit of four alternative models: (1) a model with all items loading on one "self-censorship" factor; (2) a model with one substantive "self-censorship" factor and reverse-scored items also loading on an uncorrelated method factor; (3) a model with one substantive "self-censorship" factor and two method factors (reverse-scored items loading on one method factor and straightforwardly worded items on another)ii; and (4) a model with two substantive factors and no method factors (for similar procedures see Bélanger, Caouette, Sharvit, & Dugas, 2014; Rodebaugh, Woods, & Heimberg, 2007). The fit indices of the different models are shown in Table 3.

Table 3

Fit Indices of Four Alternative Models of the Structure of the SCO Scale

Model Fit indices
N χ2 df CFI IFI NFI RMSEA
1. One substantive factor 249 524.85 77 .63 .63 .59 .15
2. One substantive factor, reverse-scored items also loading on an uncorrelated method factor 249 121.13 69 .96 .96 .91 .06
3. One substantive factor, reverse-scored items loading on one method factor and straightforwardly worded items on another 249 85.24 62 .98 .98 .93 .04
4. Two substantive factors 249 122.83 76 .96 .96 .91 .05

Note. CFI = Comparative Fit Index; IFI = Incremental Fit Index; NFI = Normative Fit Index; RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation.

The one factor model had poor fit to the data, while the three remaining models had good fit. However, in Models 2 and 3 many of the items had weak loadings on the supposed substantive factor, and none of the loadings on this factor were significant. In Model 2, the factor loadings of the reversed scored items on the supposed substantive factor ranged between -.21 and .01 (all ps > .90). In Model 3, the factor loadings of all items but one on the supposed substantive factor ranged between -.15 and .20 (all ps > .90). Conversely, in Models 2 and 3 all the loadings on the supposed method factor(s) were strong and significant. In Model 2, the factor loadings of the reversed scored items on the supposed method factor ranged between .50 and .69 (all ps > .001). In Model 3, the factor loadings of the reversed scored items on their respective method factor ranged between .53 and .73 (all ps > .001), and the factor loadings of the straightforwardly worded items on their respective factor ranged between .51 and .75 (all ps > .001). Adopting Model 2 or 3 would suggest that some or all of the items in the scale are assessing primarily method and little substance. If this were the case, there would be little point in assessing these items and we would not expect them to correlate systematically with any other measures or with expected outcomes of SCO. A more likely interpretation of these results is that the supposed "method" factors in Models 2 and 3, on which items loaded strongly, are in fact content factors. Indeed, Model 4 with two substantive content factors had good fit to the data, and in this model, all the item loadings on their respective factors were significant and greater than .50. Hence, it appears that a model with of two distinct substantive factors ("self-censorship" and "disclosure") represents the most reasonable interpretation of the findings. Model 4 is presented in Figure 1.

Inter-item correlations can be found in Appendix 1 and full CFA results in Appendix 4. Finally, we tested the internal consistency of the two factors, which was found to be high for both (self-censorship: α = .83, disclosure: α = .86).

Click to enlarge
jspp.v6i2.859-f1
Figure 1

Confirmatory factor analysis of the self-censorship orientation scale: A two-factor solution.

Note. Coefficients correspond to the arrow on their right and item numbers correspond to Table 1. All coefficients were significant (p < .05).

Correlates of SCO

Table 4 displays the zero-order correlations between the SCO factors and other measures. The two SCO factors were moderately and negatively correlated with each other. Moreover, consistent with our expectations, the self-censorship factor was related positively but weakly to WTSC, supporting our contention that the two measures are distinct. In addition, this factor was related strongly and positively to blind patriotism, conformity and RWA. The self-censorship factor was also related positively, though less strongly, to FENCE, identification as Jewish and identification with the group to which one is most attached. Finally, the self-censorship factor was negatively related to support for abstract democratic values, constructive patriotism, support for universalistic values and argumentativeness.

Table 4

Bivariate Correlations Between the Measures at Stage 1

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
1. Self-censorship
2. Disclosure -.44***
3. WTSC .17*** -.15**
4. Argumentativeness -.10* .09 -.45***
5. Fear of negative evaluation .05 -.05 .45*** -.31***
6. Identification with Jewish group .30*** -.14*** -.01 .03 .01
7. Identification with the group to which one is most attached .14** .02 -.02 .12** -.02 .44***
8. Blind patriotism .56*** -.37*** .12** -.06 .09 .48*** .21***
9. Constructive patriotism -.13** .31*** -.04 .14** .05 .03 .18*** -.03
10. Conformity .53*** -.45*** .16*** -.18*** .10* .33*** .08 .52*** -.26***
11. FENCE .39*** -.30*** .03 -.11* -.01 .28*** .07 .51*** -.20*** .49***
12. Fear .05 -.07 .32*** -.25*** .42*** -.12** -.13** -.05 -.010 .06 -.02
13. Collective Angst -.08 .08 .12* -.13** .15** -.27*** -.18*** -.22*** .05 -.15** -.15** .33***
14. RWA .56*** -.42*** .22*** -.19*** .10* .42*** .12** .61*** -.21*** .72*** .46*** .10* -.16**
15. Universalistic values -.18*** .35*** -.13** .16*** -.08 -.02 .08 -.12** .46*** -.30*** -.28*** -.06 .07 -.33***
16. Democratic values -.31*** .42*** -.07 .14** .02 -.12** .01 -.33*** .34*** -.42*** -.30*** -.07 .01 -.45*** .44***

p < .06. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

The findings regarding the disclosure factor are almost a mirror image of those regarding the self-censorship factor. Disclosure was related weakly and negatively to WTSC, supporting the distinction between the scales. In addition, disclosure was related positively to support for abstract democratic values, constructive patriotism and support for universalistic values, and marginally related to argumentativeness. Disclosure was negatively and moderately related to conformity, RWA, blind patriotism and FENCE, and negatively and weakly related to identification with the group to which one is most attached. Contrary to expectations, neither factor was related to general fear, fear of negative evaluation, or collective angst.

Given that the different measures were also highly inter-correlated, we wanted to test which of them would remain a significant predictor of the SCO factors after controlling for all others. Hence, we regressed each factor on the other measures (entered simultaneously). The results of the multiple regressions are shown in Table 5. Blind patriotism and conformity significantly predicted both factors, and were positively related to self-censorship and negatively related to disclosure. In addition, RWA and WTSC were significantly and positively related to self-censorship, but not to disclosure. Conversely, constructive patriotism, support for universalistic values and support for abstract democratic values were significantly and positively related to disclosure, but not to self-censorship. None of the other measures significantly predicted the SCO factors.

Table 5

Regression Analyses Predicting the SCO Factors by Other Measures at Stage 1

Predictor Criteria
Self-Censorship
(R2 = .41, p < .001)
Disclosure
(R2 = .31, p < .001)
B SE β p 95% Confidence Interval for B
B SE β p 95% Confidence Interval for B
Lower Upper Lower Upper
Blind patriotism .298 .047 .317 < .001 .205 .391 -.162 .043 -.203 < .001 -.246 -.077
Constructive patriotism -.035 .048 -.030 .464 -.129 .059 .138 .043 .140 .002 .053 .224
Conformity .345 .093 .195 < .001 .162 .528 -.287 .085 -.192 .001 -.453 -.120
RWA .243 .074 .191 .001 .098 .389 -.034 .067 -.031 .618 -.166 .099
WTSC .111 .056 .085 .048 .001 .222 -.092 .051 -.084 .070 -.193 .008
Universalistic values .014 .050 .012 .776 -.083 .112 .125 .045 .128 .006 .036 .213
Democratic values -.020 .052 -.017 .700 -.122 .082 .158 .047 .156 .001 .066 .251
Argumentativeness .051 .066 .031 .437 -.079 .182 -.101 .060 -.073 .093 -.219 .017
Fear of negative evaluation -.082 .052 -.067 .112 -.183 .019 .018 .047 .018 .699 -.074 .110
Identification with Jewish group .053 .037 .056 .725 -.139 .097 .022 .034 .028 .450 -.066 .148
Identification with the group to which one is most attached -.021 .060 -.016 .156 -.020 .125 .041 .054 .036 .514 -.044 .088
FENCE .063 .054 .050 .248 -.044 .169 .006 .049 .005 .911 -.091 .102
Fear .044 .056 .032 .438 -.067 .154 -.041 .051 -.036 .418 -.142 .059
Collective Angst .062 .041 .059 .128 -.018 .143 .009 .037 .010 .818 -.065 .082

SCO and Attitudes Toward Disseminating Specific Information About Ingroup and Outgroup Behaviors in the Context of an Intergroup Conflict

Next we sought to test whether the SCO factors would predict the willingness to reveal specific narrative-consistent and narrative-contradicting information about an intergroup conflict even after controlling for other measures. In order to do so, we regressed the measures of appropriateness of narrative-consistent and narrative-contradicting images on the SCO factors as well as all the other measures (entered simultaneously). The regression model for narrative-contradicting images was significant (R2 = .21, p < .001) and the disclosure factor of the SCO scale significantly predicted the rated appropriateness of these images for the EU website above and beyond the other measures (B = .15, SE = .06, β = .12, p = .014). The self-censorship factor was not a significant predictor in this model. The regression model for narrative-consistent images was significant too, but the predictors accounted only for a small percentage of the variance (R2 = .05, p < .001). This is possibly due to a ceiling effect, as most participants agreed that the narrative-consistent images were appropriate (M = 4.86, SD = 0.90). Neither factor of the SCO scale was a significant predictor of the appropriateness of narrative-consistent images. The complete results of the regression are presented in Appendix 2.

Discussion

The findings of Stage 1 demonstrated that the SCO scale is best conceptualized as a two-factor solution that consists of support for self-censorship and support for disclosure of information. Stage 1 also explored the nomological network of the two factors by examining their correlates. As predicted, the SCO factors were only weakly related to WTSC, which affirms the distinction between our conception of self-censorship and the conception previously suggested by Hayes et al. (2005a, 2005b).

The existence of two factors in the SCO scale suggests that self-censorship is a complex phenomenon involving more than a single axis on which one can be low or high. This raises the possibility that active concealment is not necessarily the direct opposite of active disclosure of information. The opposite to both of these options may be to remain passive in face of the information. In other words, when individuals receive information that they believe to be valid but potentially threatening to their group, high tendency to self-censor could lead them to actively seek to conceal the information, while high tendency to disclose could lead them to actively disseminate the information. When neither tendency is particularly high, individuals may simply do nothing with the information. It is worth noting that the two-factor structure was obtained in a sample that responded to all 31 prototype items. To strengthen our confidence in this structure, future studies could attempt to replicate it in a sample that is exposed only to the selected subset of 14 items.

In keeping with our expectations, scores on the self-censorship factor were positively related to blind patriotism, conformity, and RWA. This supports the proposition that a major motivation that drives self-censorship is the desire to protect the ingroup’s positive image and to uphold and protect group norms. Conversely, scores on the disclosure factor were positively related to constructive patriotism and to universalistic and democratic values, and negatively related to blind patriotism and conformity. This suggests that the willingness to disseminate information that might reflect negatively on one’s group could be driven by a motivation to change the group for the better and to protect the rights and welfare of all persons.

Interestingly, neither factor of the SCO scale correlated with fear or with angst. It is possible however, that fear or angst might lead to self-censorship in specific situations in which the negative consequences of revealing information are clearly evident. This direction could be explored in future research.

Overall, the results demonstrate that the SCO scale with its two factors is reliable and has good convergent and discriminant validity. There are also some preliminary indications for the scale’s predictive validity in the context of an intergroup conflict. Individuals who reported support for disclosure of information also supported presentation of images on a website that contradict the dominant narrative of the conflict and present the ingroup negatively. In sum, the results of Stage 1 support our conceptualization of self-censorship as reflected in the two factors of the SCO scale and shed light on the nomological networks of the factors.

Stage 2: Test-Retest Reliability and Predictive Validity of the SCO Scale

Stage 2 sought to investigate the test-retest reliability of the SCO factors and their ability to predict future outcomes. We hypothesized that the SCO factors would predict individuals' readiness to withhold or disseminate information about harmful acts by members of their group in the context of an intractable intergroup conflict and would do so above and beyond the other variables assessed in Stage 1.

Method

Participants

Five months after the completion of Stage 1, we attempted to reach as many of the participants as possible and asked them to complete another online questionnaire. Three hundred and eighty one out of the original 499 participants completed Stage 2 (re-response rate of 76%). Participants who completed Stage 2 had the following socio-demographic characteristics: Mean age 40.41 years (SD = 13.78); 49% female; 54% considered themselves secular, 23% were observant, 16% were religious and 7% were Ultra-Orthodox; 29% had at least a college degree; 38% had some college or professional education, and 33% had 12 or fewer years of schooling; 57% regarded themselves as having a rightist political orientation; 25% regarded themselves as centrists, and 18% regarded themselves as leftists. Apart from a slightly higher average age, there were no significant differences in socio-demographic characteristics between the Stage 2 sub-sample and the full sample of Stage 1.

Measures

Disseminating information about harmful behaviors by ingroup members

Participants were presented with a brief paragraph used in previous research (Roccas, Klar, & Liviatan, 2006), which described a historical event from the Israeli-Palestinian conflict in which the Israeli Border Patrol shot and killed 43 Arab civilians in Kafer Kassem in 1956. There is a great degree of consensus among Jewish Israelis that in this particular event the Border Patrol soldiers had committed a serious legal and moral violation. After reading the event description, participants responded to two items (α = .72) referring to the importance of concealing information about the Kafer Kassem killing from the public (e.g., "even if this event actually happened, exposing it to the general public compromises the security of the state"), and one item referring to the importance of revealing information about this event (i.e., "exposing this event and similar events is important in order to learn lessons and prevent their recurrence"). Responses ranged between 1 (do not agree at all) and 7 (agree completely).

SCO scale

After completing the measure regarding dissemination of information, participants completed the 14-item SCO scale as developed in Stage 1 (self-censorship: α = .84, disclosure: α = .90).

Results

The cross-temporal stability of the SCO factors at Stage 1 and Stage 2 was estimated using correlational analyses. Results revealed moderate stability of the factors over time. The test-retest reliability of the self-censorship factor was .61 and that of the disclosure factor was .56.

Next, we tested whether the SCO factors as assessed at Stage 1 would predict attitudes toward withholding or disseminating information regarding harmful behaviors by ingroup members, assessed at Stage 2. We regressed the Stage 2 measures of readiness to withhold or disseminate information about the Kafer Kassem killing on the SCO factors as well as all other measures assessed at Stage 1 (entered simultaneously). The overall regression models for both criteria were significant (withhold information: R2 = .29, p < .001, disseminate information: R2 = .24, p < .001). More importantly, the self-censorship factor of the SCO scale significantly predicted the readiness to withhold information about the Kafer Kassem killing above and beyond all the other Stage 1 measures (B = .20, SE = .09, β = .12, p = .036). The disclosure factor of the SCO scale did not significantly predict this outcome (B = -.14, SE = .11, β = -.07, p = .208). However, the disclosure factor significantly predicted the readiness to disseminate information about the Kafer Kassem killing above and beyond all other measures (B = .33, SE = .11, β = .17, p = .003), whereas the self-censorship factor did not significantly predict this outcome (B = -.02, SE = .10, β = -.01, p = .811). The complete results of the regression are presented in Appendix 3.

Discussion

In line with our expectations, participants’ SCO factor scores at Stage 1 were robustly correlated with their SCO factor scores at Stage 2. Furthermore, Stage 2 demonstrated the predictive validity of the SCO factors. In keeping with the theoretical definition of self-censorship, we found that the higher one’s self-censorship score, the more one is ready to withhold information that impinges negatively on the group’s image in the context of an intergroup conflict five months later. In contrast, the higher one’s disclosure score, the more one is willing to reveal information that portrays the ingroup negatively in the context of a conflict five months later. The findings showing that each of the two factors in the SCO scale predicted different conflict-related outcomes at Stage 2 are consistent with the interpretation of the two factors as representing distinct dimensions rather than polar opposites. In general, the results validate the contention that support for self-censorship or disclosure of information is a stable individual characteristic with predictive validity for future outcomes related to the readiness to withhold or disseminate information in the context of an intractable violent conflict.

General Discussion

The present research sought to develop a new instrument to assess individuals’ orientation toward self-censorship of information, examine this construct in relation to relevant constructs and demonstrate its predictive utility, particularly in the context of a violent intractable conflict. Our measure focused on self-censorship or disclosure of information that is considered valid and that may have implications for one’s ingroup. Factor analyses revealed that individuals’ orientations toward restricting or expanding the dissemination of such information vary along two distinct but related dimensions. The first dimension, labeled “self-censorship”, reflects the tendency to conceal information that is seen as threatening. The second dimension, labeled “disclosure”, reflects the tendency to disseminate critical information. While the two dimensions appear to be polar opposites, the two-factor solution that emerged both in exploratory and in confirmatory factor analysis suggests that this is not the case. The emergence of two factors suggests the possibility that both dimensions reflect taking some kind of action regarding the information that one possesses, whether the action aims to conceal the information or reveal it. The low end of each dimension may reflect a preference to remain passive and not take any action to reveal or conceal the information. Interestingly, Timming and Johnstone (2015) make a similar observation regarding silence and voice in organizational contexts:

…it remains unclear whether silence should be viewed as the opposite of voice…On the one hand, it is possible that employees make a simple choice between expressing their views (voice) or keeping quiet (silence)… On the other hand, while voice can be viewed as a deliberate choice, silence might not be a conscious decision, but the result of more general feelings of disengagement, psychological withdrawal or simply having nothing to say. The absence of intentional silence, defined as the deliberate withholding of information, does not mean the presence of voice behavior. (p. 157)

Our findings also confirmed that our measure of SCO is distinct from other measures of similar constructs. Importantly, the findings indicate that the two SCO factors had only low correlations with the WTSC measure developed by Hayes et al. (2005a, 2005b). This affirms the distinction between our conception, which focuses on self-censorship of information, and the definition proposed by Hayes et al. (2005a), which focuses on self-censorship of opinions. It is worth noting that measures of constructs similar to self-censorship, such as silence, were also developed for organizational settings (Brinsfield, 2013; Knoll & van Dick, 2013). However, these measures ask respondents to refer specifically to work or organization settings, whereas our measure is applicable to many different settings outside of the organizational context. Because our focus in the present research was on implications of SCO in the context of large-scale intergroup conflicts, we did not include measures referring to organizational or work contexts in our research.

Our findings also indicated that the SCO factors had moderate correlations with conformity, supporting our contention that although related, self-censorship and conformity are distinct phenomena. Furthermore, we proposed that a central motivation for self-censorship of information, other than self-interest, is protection of the group to which one belongs. In keeping with this expectation, we found that the self-censorship dimension was positively related, and the disclosure dimension negatively related, to variables reflecting commitment to one’s group, such as blind patriotism, and to variables reflecting conservatism and adherence to accepted ideas and norms, such as RWA. Conversely, the self-censorship factor was negatively related, and the disclosure dimension positively related, to variables reflecting commitment to freedom of information and expression, such as universalistic and democratic values, and to variables reflecting willingness to be critical of one’s group, such as constructive patriotism.

Finally, our findings affirm the predictive validity of the SCO scale dimensions. The disclosure dimension predicted both cross-sectionally and longitudinally the willingness to disseminate information that was critical of one’s ingroup in the context of a conflict and portrayed it negatively. The self-censorship dimension predicted the willingness to conceal such information longitudinally (but not cross-sectionally). Both dimensions predicted their respective outcomes above and beyond all other variables assessed at Stage 1. Hence, the SCO scale may be a useful in predicting individuals’ intensions to conceal or reveal information that may have negative implications for their group, particularly in the context of an intergroup conflict. Future research may extend the investigation of the scale’s predictive validity by testing whether it can predict actual behavior of concealing or revealing information. It should be noted, however, that the readiness to conceal or reveal information in any particular situation may be a function not only of one’s general disposition, but also of various situational factors, including, for example, the nature of the information, the role and accountability of the person possessing the information, and the role(s) of the person(s) to whom the information might be revealed (Shahar et al., 2018). Future research may expand our understanding of self-censorship and disclosure of information by investigating the conditions under which each is more or less likely to occur (for further discussion see Sharvit, 2017).

Some notable limitations of the present research could also be addressed in future studies. First, the current findings are correlational and causality cannot be inferred. Given the nature of self-censorship and disclosure orientations as individual dispositions, it would be difficult to establish causality through an experimental design. However, a cross-lagged longitudinal design could help strengthen the case for causality. Second, the present study focused on the implications of self-censorship and disclosure orientations in a context of an intergroup conflict. It remains unknown whether these orientations can also predict the willingness to withhold or disclose information in other social contexts. Future studies could investigate whether the implications of self-censorship and disclosure orientations extend beyond the context of intergroup conflict.

Self-censorship may have serious consequences for the well-being of individuals and groups and for the functioning of societies. On the one hand, it may contribute to social cohesion and to mobilization toward important social causes. It may also allow societal institutions to function freely without being subject to constant scrutiny and criticism. But on the other hand, self-censorship may interfere with the free flow of information in a society, which can hinder free and open social debate of controversial issues and reduce transparency and accountability in social institutions. The present research focused on the consequences of self-censorship in a context of intergroup conflict, demonstrating that the tendency to self-censor predicts reluctance to expose information that portrays the ingroup’s conduct negatively, whereas the tendency to disclose predicts willingness to reveal and disseminate such information. These processes may contribute to the formation and maintenance of a conflict-supporting social narrative, which can become a barrier to peaceful conflict resolution (Hameiri, Bar-Tal, et al., 2017; Hameiri, Sharvit, et al., 2017).

However, self-censorship may also have important implications in other social domains, for example in the context of good governance and prevention of corruption, or in the legal context, where withholding of evidence can bias the judicial process. Future studies can continue to investigate the implications of self-censorship in additional contexts. The measure developed in the present research provides a reliable and valid instrument that may be useful in such future investigations of theoretically driven hypotheses regarding the social consequences of self-censorship.

Notes

i) We do not maintain that an absolutely valid truth exists in every case. Our focus is on individuals’ belief that their knowledge is valid.

ii) The method factors were allowed to correlate with each other but not with the substantive factor.

Funding

The research was funded by Israeli Science Foundation (ISF) grant #1231/11 rewarded to the second author. The third author is grateful to the Azrieli Foundation for the award of an Azrieli Fellowship.

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Acknowledgments

The authors have no support to report.

References

  • Altemeyer, B. (1996). The authoritarian specter. Cambridge, MA, USA: Harvard University Press.

  • Antilla, L. (2010). Self-censorship and science: A geographical review of media coverage of climate tipping points. Public Understanding of Science, 19, 240-256. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662508094099

  • Bagozzi, R. P., & Yi, Y. (1990). Assessing method variance in multitrait-multimethod matrices: The case of self-reported affect and perceptions at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75(5), 547-560. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.75.5.547

  • Barendt, E. (2005). Freedom of speech. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.

  • Bar-Tal, D. (2013). Intractable conflicts: Sociopsychological foundations and dynamics. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.

  • Bar-Tal, D. (2017). Self-censorship as a socio-political-psychological phenomenon: Conception and research. Advances in Political Psychology, 38, 37-65. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12391

  • Bar-Tal, D., Nets-Zehngut, R., & Sharvit, K. (Eds.). (2017). Self-censorship in contexts of conflict: Theory and research. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

  • Bar-Tal, D., Oren, N., & Nets-Zehngut, R. (2014). Sociopsychological analysis of conflict-supporting narratives: A general framework. Journal of Peace Research, 51(5), 662-675. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022343314533984

  • Bélanger, J. J., Caouette, J., Sharvit, K., & Dugas, M. (2014). The psychology of martyrdom: Making the ultimate sacrifice in the name of a cause. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(3), 494-515. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036855

  • Biderman, M. D., Nguyen, N. T., Cunningham, C. J., & Ghorbani, N. (2011). The ubiquity of common method variance: The case of the Big Five. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 417-429. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2011.05.001

  • Brinsfield, C. T. (2013). Employee silence motives: Investigation of dimensionality and development of measures. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 34(5), 671-697. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.1829

  • Brown-Smith, N. (1998). Family secrets. Journal of Family Issues, 19, 20-42. https://doi.org/10.1177/019251398019001003

  • Central Bureau of Statistics. (2013). Statistical abstract of Israel. Jerusalem, Israel: Central Bureau of Statistics.

  • Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591-621. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015

  • David, O., & Bar-Tal, D. (2009). A socio-psychological conception of collective identity: The case of national identity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(4), 354-379. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309344412

  • De Baets, A. (2002). Censorship of historical thought: A world guide, 1945-2000. Westport, CT, USA: Greenwood Press.

  • Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 629-636. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046408

  • Duckitt, J., & Sibley, C. G. (2009). A dual-process motivational model of ideology, politics, and prejudice. Psychological Inquiry, 20, 98-109. https://doi.org/10.1080/10478400903028540

  • Feldman, S. (2003). Enforcing social conformity: A theory of authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 24, 41-74. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00316

  • Feldman, S., & Stenner, K. (1997). Perceived threat and authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 18, 741-770. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00077

  • Gundlach, M. J., Douglas, S. C., & Martinko, M. J. (2003). The decision to blow the whistle: A social information processing framework. Academy of Management Review, 28, 107-123. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.2003.8925239

  • Halperin, E., & Bar-Tal, D. (2011). Socio-psychological barriers to peace making: An empirical examination within the Israeli Jewish Society. Journal of Peace Research, 48, 637-651. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022343311412642

  • Hameiri, B., Bar-Tal, D., & Halperin, E. (2017). Self-censorship as a socio-psychological barrier to peacemaking. In D. Bar-Tal, R. Nets-Zehngut, & K. Sharvit (Eds.), Self-censorship in contexts of conflict: Theory and research (pp. 61-78). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

  • Hameiri, B., Sharvit, K., Bar-Tal, D., Shahar, E., & Halperin, E. (2017). Support for self-censorship among Israelis as a barrier to resolving the Israeli-Palestinian conflict. Political Psychology, 38(5), 795-813. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12346

  • Hayes, A. F., Glynn, C. J., & Shanahan, J. (2005a). Willingness to self-censor: A construct and measurement tool for public opinion research. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 17, 298-323. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edh073

  • Hayes, A. F., Glynn, C. J., & Shanahan, J. (2005b). Validating the Willingness to Self-Censor Scale: Individual differences in the effect of the climate of opinion on opinion expression. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 17, 443-455. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edh072

  • Hayes, A. F., Uldall, B. R., & Glynn, C. J. (2010). Validating the Willingness to Self-Censor Scale II: Inhibition of opinion expression in a conversational setting. Communication Methods and Measures, 4, 256-272. https://doi.org/10.1080/19312458.2010.505503

  • Henik, E. (2015). Understanding whistle-blowing: A set-theoretic approach. Journal of Business Research, 68(2), 442-450. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.06.004

  • Huddy, L., Feldman, S., & Cassese, E. (2007). On the distinct political effects of anxiety and anger. In A. Crigler, M. MacKuen, G. E. Marcus, & W. R. Neuman (Eds.), The affect effect: Dynamics of emotion in political thinking and behavior (pp. 202-230). Chicago, IL, USA: University of Chicago Press.

  • Infante, D. A., & Rancer, A. S. (1982). A conceptualization and measure of argumentativeness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 46, 72-80. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4601_13

  • Jansen, S. C. (1988). Censorship: The knot that binds power and knowledge. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.

  • Jos, P. H., Tompkins, M. E., & Hays, S. W. (1989). In praise of difficult people: A portrait of the committed whistleblower. Public Administration Review, 49(6), 552-561. https://doi.org/10.2307/976577

  • Karpel, M. A. (1980). Family secrets. Family Process, 19, 295-306. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1980.00295.x

  • Kenny, T., & Gross, P. (2008). Journalism in Central Asia: A victim of politics, economics, and widespread self-censorship. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 13, 515-525. https://doi.org/10.1177/1940161208324644

  • Klar, Y., & Baram, H. (2016). In DeFENCE of the in-group historical narrative in an intractable intergroup conflict: An individual-difference perspective. Political Psychology, 37(1), 37-53. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12229

  • Knoll, M., & van Dick, R. (2013). Do I hear the whistle…? A first attempt to measure four forms of employee silence and their correlates. Journal of Business Ethics, 113(2), 349-362. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-012-1308-4

  • Leary, M. R. (1983). A brief version of the fear of negative evaluation scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 371-375. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167283093007

  • Lee, F. L., & Chan, J. (2009). Organizational production of self-censorship in the Hong Kong media. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 14, 112-133. https://doi.org/10.1177/1940161208326598

  • MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Zhang, S., & Hong, S. (1999). Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological Methods, 4(1), 84-99. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.4.1.84

  • Maksudyan, N. (2009). Walls of silence: Translating the Armenian genocide into Turkish and self-censorship. Critique, 37, 635-649. https://doi.org/10.1080/03017600903205781

  • Marsh, H. W., Scalas, L. F., & Nagengast, B. (2010). Longitudinal tests of competing factor structures for the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Traits, ephemeral artifacts, and stable response styles. Psychological Assessment, 22, 366-381. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019225

  • Morrison, E. W., & Milliken, F. J. (2000). Organizational silence: A barrier to change and development in a pluralistic world. Academy of Management Review, 25, 706-725. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.2000.3707697

  • Near, J. P., & Miceli, M. P. (1996). Whistle-blowing: Myth and reality. Journal of Management, 22, 507-526. https://doi.org/10.1177/014920639602200306

  • Nelkin, D. (1995). Selling science: How the press covers science and technology (revised ed.). New York, NY, USA: W.H. Freeman & Co.

  • Nets-Zehngut, R., Pliskin, R., & Bar-Tal, D. (2015). Self-censorship in conflicts: Israel and the 1948 Palestinian exodus. Peace and Conflict, 21(3), 479-499. https://doi.org/10.1037/pac0000094

  • Nikolaou, I., Vakola, M., & Bourantas, D. (2008). Who speaks up at work? Dispositional influences on employees’ voice behavior. Personnel Review, 37(6), 666-679. https://doi.org/10.1108/00483480810906892

  • Packer, D. J. (2008). On being both with us and against us: A normative conflict model of dissent in social groups. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12(1), 50-72. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868307309606

  • Petronio, S. (2010). Communication privacy management theory: What do we know about family privacy regulation? Journal of Family Theory & Review, 2, 175-196. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-2589.2010.00052.x

  • Premeaux, S. F., & Bedeian, A. G. (2003). Breaking the silence: The moderating effects of self-monitoring in predicting speaking up in the workplace. Journal of Management Studies, 40(6), 1537-1562. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6486.00390

  • Roccas, S., Klar, Y., & Liviatan, I. (2006). The paradox of group-based guilt: Modes of national identification, conflict vehemence, and reactions to the in-group’s moral violations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 698-711. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.698

  • Rodebaugh, T. L., Woods, C. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (2007). The reverse of social anxiety is not always the opposite: The reverse-scored items of the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale do not belong. Behavior Therapy, 38, 192-206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006.08.001

  • Schatz, R. T., Staub, E., & Lavine, H. (1999). On the varieties of national attachment: Blind versus constructive patriotism. Political Psychology, 20, 151-174. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00140

  • Schauer, F. (1982). Codifying the First Amendment: New York v. Ferber. The Supreme Court Review, 1982, 285-317. https://doi.org/10.1086/scr.1982.3109559

  • Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theory and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1-65). New York, NY, USA: Academic Press.

  • Schwartz, S. H. (2007). Universalism values and the inclusiveness of our moral universe. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 711-728. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022107308992

  • Shahar, E., Hameiri, B., Bar-Tal, D., & Raviv, A. (2018). Self-censorship of conflict-related information in the context of intractable conflict. The Journal of Conflict Resolution, 62(5), 957-982. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022002716680266

  • Shamir, M. (1991). Political intolerance among masses and elites in Israel: A reevaluation of the elitist theory of democracy. The Journal of Politics, 53, 1018-1043. https://doi.org/10.2307/2131865

  • Shamir, M., & Sullivan, J. L. (1985). Jews and Arabs in Israel: Everybody hates somebody, sometime. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 29, 283-305. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022002785029002006

  • Sharvit, K. (2017). Self-censorship: Emerging themes and remaining questions. In D. Bar-Tal, R. Nets-Zehngut, & K. Sharvit (Eds.), Self-censorship in contexts of conflict: Theory and research (pp. 269-277). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

  • Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30(4), 526-537. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0037039

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1989). Using multivariate statistics (2nd ed.). Cambridge, United Kingdom: Harper & Row.

  • Tajfel, H. (1978). Social categorization, social identity and social comparison. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social groups (pp. 61-76). London, United Kingdom: Academic Press.

  • Tangirala, S., Kamdar, D., Venkataramani, V., & Parke, M. R. (2013). Doing right versus getting ahead: The effects of duty and achievement orientations on employees’ voice. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(6), 1040-1050. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033855

  • Timming, A. R., & Johnstone, S. (2015). Employee silence and authoritarian personality: A political psychology of workplace democracy. The International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 23(1), 154-171. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJOA-06-2013-0685

  • Tribe, D. (1973). Questions of censorship. New York, NY, USA: St. Martin’s Press.

  • Van Dyne, L., Ang, S., & Botero, I. C. (2003). Conceptualizing employee silence and employee voice as multidimensional constructs. Journal of Management Studies, 40(6), 1359-1392. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6486.00384

  • Vangelisti, A. L. (1994). Family secrets: Forms, functions and correlates. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 113-135. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407594111007

  • Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1999). The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule-expanded form. Iowa City, IA, USA: University of Iowa, Department of Psychology.

  • Westin, A. F. (Ed.). (1981). Whistle blowing. New York, NY, USA: McGraw-Hill.

  • Wohl, M. J., & Branscombe, N. R. (2009). Group threat, collective angst, and ingroup forgiveness for the war in Iraq. Political Psychology, 30, 193-217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00688.x

  • Wohl, M. J., Branscombe, N. R., & Reysen, S. (2010). Perceiving your group’s future to be in jeopardy: Extinction threat induces collective angst and the desire to strengthen the ingroup. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 898-910. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210372505

Appendices

Appendix 1: Correlations Among the Self-Censorship Scale Items (Stage 1)

Item 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13
Item 1
Item 2 .51**
Item 3 .51** .47**
Item 4 .51** .49** .48**
Item 5 .43** .37** .37** .33**
Item 6 .37** .37** .44** .40** .37**
Item 7 .42** .46** .42** .44** .35** .41**
Item 8 .36** .56** .48** .42** .37** .46** .44**
Item 9 -.06 -.23** -.17** -.17** -.17** -.16* -.13* -.18**
Item 10 -.03 -.15* -.15* -.19** -.17** -.14* -.13* -.20** .56**
Item 11 -.03 -.15* -.17** -.19** -.09 -.09 -.11 -.13* .47** .56**
Item 12 -.19** -.26** -.18** -.20** -.26** -.20** -.15* -.19** .64** .56** .51**
Item 13 -.15* -.17** -.18** -.23** -.21** -.18** -.17** -.14* .42** .32** .42** .55**
Item 14 -.10 -.17** -.14* -.13* -.13* -.19** -.30** -.19** .43** .32** .35** .42** .33**

Note. Item numbers correspond to Table 2 in the main text.

*p < .05. **p < .001.

Appendix 2: Regression Analyses Predicting Support for Disseminating Information About Ingroup and Outgroup Behavior by all Other Measures at Stage 1

Predictor Criteria
Narrative-Consistent Images
Narrative-Contradicting Images
B SE β B SE β
Self-Censorship -.001 .05 -.001 .03 .06 .03
Disclosure .03 .05 .03 .15 .06 .12*
WTSC -.01 .06 -.01 -.01 .07 -.01
Argumentativeness .03 .05 .04 .10 .06 .08
Fear of negative evaluation .03 .05 .03 -.10 .07 -.08
Identification with Jewish group .12 .06 .12* -.09 .07 -.06
Identification with the group to which one is most attached .05 .04 .07 .01 .05 .01
Blind patriotism .07 .05 .09 -.17 .06 -.18*
Constructive patriotism -.02 .05 -.03 -.01 .06 -.004
Conformity -.15 .10 -.11 .07 .12 .04
FENCE .13 .06 .13* -.30 .07 -.23**
Fear .08 .06 .07 .13 .07 .09
Collective angst .03 .04 .03 .01 .05 .01
RWA .04 .08 .04 -.01 .09 -.01
Universalistic values .08 .05 .08 .04 .06 .03
Democratic values -.07 .05 -.08 .15 .06 .12*

*p < .05. **p < .001.

Appendix 3: Regression Analyses Predicting Support for Withholding or Disseminating Information About the Kafer Kassem Killing at Stage 2 by All Stage 1 Measures

Predictor Criteria
Withhold information
Disseminate information
B SE β B SE β
Self-Censorship .20 .09 .12* -.02 .10 -.01
Disclosure -.14 .11 -.07 .33 .11 .17*
WTSC .10 .12 .05 -.03 .12 -.01
Argumentativeness -.08 .14 -.03 -.08 .14 -.03
Fear of negative evaluation -.18 .11 -.09 .27 .11 .13*
Identification with Jewish group -.09 .13 -.04 .16 .13 .07
Identification with the group to which one is most attached < .001 .08 < .001 -.05 .08 -.03
Blind patriotism .44 .11 .29** -.24 .11 -.16*
Constructive patriotism -.02 .10 -.01 .06 .11 .03
Conformity -.10 .20 -.04 -.08 .21 -.03
FENCE -.03 .10 -.02 -.004 .10 -.002
Fear .03 .12 .01 -.09 .12 -.04
Collective angst .11 .09 .06 .05 .09 .03
RWA .38 .16 .18* -.41 .17 -.20*
Universalistic values .07 .11 .03 .20 .11 .11
Democratic values -.20 .11 -.10 .06 .11 .03

*p < .05. **p < .001.

Appendix 4: Detailed Information About Confirmatory Factor Analysis Models

Note: Item numbers correspond to Table 1 in the main text.

Model 1 – One Substantive Factor

Regression Weights

Estimate SE Standardized Estimate p
ITEM 1 Self-Censorship factor 1.000 .616
ITEM 2 Self-Censorship factor 1.129 .129 .699 < .001
ITEM 3 Self-Censorship factor 1.214 .143 .664 < .001
ITEM 4 Self-Censorship factor 1.191 .142 .658 < .001
ITEM 5 Self-Censorship factor .987 .135 .550 < .001
ITEM 6 Self-Censorship factor 1.131 .146 .590 < .001
ITEM 7 Self-Censorship factor 1.027 .128 .618 < .001
ITEM 8 Self-Censorship factor 1.166 .139 .660 < .001
ITEM 9 Self-Censorship factor -.803 .144 -.402 < .001
ITEM 10 Self-Censorship factor -.713 .139 -.366 < .001
ITEM 11 Self-Censorship factor -.649 .136 -.337 < .001
ITEM 12 Self-Censorship factor -.861 .137 -.462 < .001
ITEM 13 Self-Censorship factor -.722 .134 -.388 < .001
ITEM 14 Self-Censorship factor -.808 .160 -.360 < .001

Variances

Estimate SE p
Self-Censorship factor .566 .112 < .001
Error 1 .924 .092 < .001
Error 2 .757 .080 < .001
Error 3 1.058 .108 < .001
Error 4 1.051 .107 < .001
Error 5 1.272 .122 < .001
Error 6 1.353 .132 < .001
Error 7 .968 .096 < .001
Error 8 .999 .102 < .001
Error 9 1.896 .175 < .001
Error 10 1.859 .171 < .001
Error 11 1.854 .170 < .001
Error 12 1.549 .145 < .001
Error 13 1.670 .154 < .001
Error 14 2.478 .228 < .001

Squared Multiple Correlations

Estimate
ITEM 14 .130
ITEM 13 .150
ITEM 12 .213
ITEM 11 .114
ITEM 10 .134
ITEM 9 .161
ITEM 8 .435
ITEM 7 .382
ITEM 6 .348
ITEM 5 .302
ITEM 4 .433
ITEM 3 .441
ITEM 2 .488
ITEM 1 .380

Model 2 – One Substantive Factor, Reverse-Scored Items Also Loading on an Uncorrelated Method Factor

Regression Weights

Estimate SE Standardized Estimate p
ITEM 1 Self-Censorship factor 1.000 .007
ITEM 2 Self-Censorship factor -30.969 314.597 -.216 .922
ITEM 3 Self-Censorship factor -27.133 276.798 -.167 .922
ITEM 4 Self-Censorship factor -30.670 311.925 -.190 .922
ITEM 5 Self-Censorship factor -33.325 337.389 -.208 .921
ITEM 6 Self-Censorship factor -30.274 307.651 -.177 .922
ITEM 7 Self-Censorship factor -22.629 230.869 -.153 .922
ITEM 8 Self-Censorship factor -30.171 306.784 -.192 .922
ITEM 9 Self-Censorship factor 137.552 1376.397 .765 .920
ITEM 10 Self-Censorship factor 122.420 1224.985 .699 .920
ITEM 11 Self-Censorship factor 114.481 1145.553 .662 .920
ITEM 13 Self-Censorship factor 99.502 995.678 .594 .920
ITEM 14 Self-Censorship factor 105.128 1051.993 .521 .920
ITEM 12 Self-Censorship factor 137.326 1374.122 .819 .920
ITEM 1 Method factor 1.000 .688
ITEM 2 Method factor .971 .103 .680 < .001
ITEM 3 Method factor 1.083 .118 .669 < .001
ITEM 4 Method factor 1.032 .115 .644 < .001
ITEM 5 Method factor .797 .112 .501 < .001
ITEM 6 Method factor .958 .121 .564 < .001
ITEM 7 Method factor .899 .106 .609 < .001
ITEM 8 Method factor 1.000 .112 .639 < .001

Variances

Estimate SE p
Self-Censorship factor .000 .001 .960
Method factor .707 .123 < .001
Error 1 .785 .085 < .001
Error 2 .709 .077 < .001
Error 3 .974 .103 < .001
Error 4 .997 .104 < .001
Error 5 1.263 .121 < .001
Error 6 1.327 .131 < .001
Error 7 .933 .094 < .001
Error 8 .960 .099 < .001
Error 9 .935 .110 < .001
Error 10 1.097 .117 < .001
Error 11 1.174 .121 < .001
Error 12 .648 .088 < .001
Error 13 1.272 .125 < .001
Error 14 2.073 .198 < .001

Squared Multiple Correlations

Estimate
ITEM 14 .272
ITEM 13 .353
ITEM 12 .671
ITEM 11 .438
ITEM 10 .489
ITEM 9 .586
ITEM 8 .445
ITEM 7 .394
ITEM 6 .350
ITEM 5 .294
ITEM 4 .451
ITEM 3 .475
ITEM 2 .509
ITEM 1 .474

Model 3 – One Substantive Factor, Reverse-Scored Items Loading on One Method Factor and Straightforwardly Worded Items on Another

Regression Weights

Estimate SE Standardized Estimate p
ITEM 1 Self-Censorship factor 1.000 .668
ITEM 2 Self-Censorship factor .111 .266 .074 .676
ITEM 3 Self-Censorship factor .205 .270 .121 .449
ITEM 4 Self-Censorship factor .244 .254 .146 .335
ITEM 5 Self-Censorship factor .226 .215 .137 .293
ITEM 6 Self-Censorship factor -.033 .313 -.019 .916
ITEM 7 Self-Censorship factor .044 .260 .028 .867
ITEM 8 Self-Censorship factor -.248 .410 -.152 .546
ITEM 9 Self-Censorship factor .269 .283 .146 .342
ITEM 10 Self-Censorship factor .362 .307 .202 .238
ITEM 11 Self-Censorship factor .264 .260 .149 .311
ITEM 12 Self-Censorship factor -.008 .188 -.004 .968
ITEM 13 Self-Censorship factor -.021 .161 -.012 .897
ITEM 14 Self-Censorship factor .197 .243 .095 .418
ITEM 1 Method factor 1 1.000 .628
ITEM 2 Method factor 1 1.140 .296 .719 < .001
ITEM 3 Method factor 1 1.215 .296 .678 < .001
ITEM 4 Method factor 1 1.161 .271 .654 < .001
ITEM 5 Method factor 1 .938 .223 .533 < .001
ITEM 6 Method factor 1 1.147 .343 .610 < .001
ITEM 7 Method factor 1 1.032 .288 .633 < .001
ITEM 8 Method factor 1 1.271 .427 .733 .003
ITEM 9 Method factor 2 .957 .083 .749 < .001
ITEM 10 Method factor 2 .841 .085 .676 < .001
ITEM 11 Method factor 2 .793 .081 .645 < .001
ITEM 12 Method factor 2 1.000 .839
ITEM 13 Method factor 2 .719 .075 .604 < .001
ITEM 14 Method factor 2 .736 .095 .514 < .001

Covariances

Estimate SE Correlation p
Method factor 1 Method factor 2 -.337 .103 -.373 .001

Variances

Estimate SE p
Self-Censorship factor .666 .458 .147
Method factor 1 .588 .329 .074
Method factor 2 1.385 .184 < .001
Error 1 .236 .271 .384
Error 2 .706 .076 < .001
Error 3 .994 .102 < .001
Error 4 1.022 .103 < .001
Error 5 1.271 .121 < .001
Error 6 1.303 .132 < .001
Error 7 .937 .095 < .001
Error 8 .776 .110 < .001
Error 9 .943 .109 < .001
Error 10 1.079 .116 < .001
Error 11 1.175 .121 < .001
Error 12 .584 .089 < .001
Error 13 1.249 .124 < .001
Error 14 2.070 .197 < .001

Squared Multiple Correlations

Estimate
ITEM 14 .273
ITEM 13 .365
ITEM 12 .704
ITEM 11 .438
ITEM 10 .497
ITEM 9 .583
ITEM 8 .561
ITEM 7 .401
ITEM 6 .373
ITEM 5 .302
ITEM 4 .449
ITEM 3 .474
ITEM 2 .523
ITEM 1 .841

Model 4 – Two Substantive Factors

Regression Weights

Estimate SE Standardized Estimate p
ITEM 1 Disclosure factor 1.000 .670
ITEM 2 Disclosure factor 1.074 .110 .723 < .001
ITEM 3 Disclosure factor 1.167 .124 .694 < .001
ITEM 4 Disclosure factor 1.130 .122 .679 < .001
ITEM 5 Disclosure factor .910 .118 .552 < .001
ITEM 6 Disclosure factor 1.062 .127 .603 < .001
ITEM 7 Disclosure factor .972 .111 .636 < .001
ITEM 8 Disclosure factor 1.106 .119 .681 < .001
ITEM 9 Self-Censorship factor .987 .080 .761 < .001
ITEM 10 Self-Censorship factor .874 .079 .692 < .001
ITEM 11 Self-Censorship factor .821 .078 .659 < .001
ITEM 12 Self-Censorship factor 1.000 .827
ITEM 13 Self-Censorship factor .723 .077 .599 < .001
ITEM 14 Self-Censorship factor .759 .094 .522 < .001

Covariances

Estimate SE Correlation p
Disclosure factor Self-Censorship factor -.335 .078 -.353 < .001

Variances

Estimate SE p
Disclosure factor .670 .119 < .001
Self-Censorship factor 1.347 .180 < .001
Error 1 .820 .085 < .001
Error 2 .706 .077 < .001
Error 3 .979 .103 < .001
Error 4 .999 .104 < .001
Error 5 1.268 .122 < .001
Error 6 1.321 .130 < .001
Error 7 .932 .094 < .001
Error 8 .949 .099 < .001
Error 9 .950 .111 < .001
Error 10 1.118 .118 < .001
Error 11 1.184 .121 < .001
Error 12 .622 .087 < .001
Error 13 1.261 .125 < .001
Error 14 2.071 .198 < .001

Squared Multiple Correlations

Estimate
ITEM 14 .273
ITEM 13 .358
ITEM 12 .684
ITEM 11 .434
ITEM 10 .479
ITEM 9 .580
ITEM 8 .463
ITEM 7 .404
ITEM 6 .364
ITEM 5 .304
ITEM 4 .461
ITEM 3 .482
ITEM 2 .523
ITEM 1 .449